NC Research Report Discussion

  • Discuss the ways in which a research report is similar to and different from the customary term paper.
  • Think about the various sections of the research report. What might be the most difficult section to write, and which is the easiest? Why?
  • First, read the Youngs et al. (2021) article. Then respond to the following question.
  • Take a look at the methods section. Be critical of their methods- was there anything they did exceptionally well? Were there any limitations to their research?

    Cognition, Language, and Development
    Mindfulness Meditation
    Improves Visual
    Short-Term Memory
    Psychological Reports
    2021, Vol. 124(4) 1673–1686
    ! The Author(s) 2020
    Article reuse guidelines:
    sagepub.com/journals-permissions
    DOI: 10.1177/0033294120926670
    journals.sagepub.com/home/prx
    Molly A. Youngs, Samuel E. Lee, and
    Michael O. Mireku
    School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Lincoln, UK
    Dinkar Sharma
    School of Psychology, University of Kent, Canterbury, UK
    Robin S. S. Kramer
    School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Lincoln, UK
    Abstract
    Research into the effects of mindfulness meditation on behavioral outcomes has
    received much interest in recent years, with benefits for both short-term memory
    and working memory identified. However, little research has considered the potential effects of brief mindfulness meditation interventions or the nature of any benefits
    for visual short-term memory. Here, we investigate the effect of a single, 8-minute
    mindfulness meditation intervention, presented via audio recording, on a short-term
    memory task for faces. In comparison with two control groups (listening to an
    audiobook or simply passing the time however they wished), our mindfulness meditation participants showed greater increases in visual short-term memory capacity
    from pre- to post-intervention. In addition, only mindfulness meditation resulted in
    significant increases in performance. In conclusion, a single, brief mindfulness meditation intervention led to improvements in visual short-term memory capacity for
    faces, with important implications regarding the minimum intervention necessary to
    produce measurable changes in short-term memory tasks.
    Corresponding Author:
    Robin S. S. Kramer, School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Lincoln LN6 7TS, UK.
    Email: remarknibor@gmail.com
    1674
    Psychological Reports 124(4)
    Keywords
    Mindfulness, meditation, visual short-term memory, faces, intervention
    Introduction
    Working memory (WM) is a multicomponent system, bringing together shortterm memory (STM) and attention (Cowan, 2016), and has a limited capacity,
    varying between individuals (Ortells et al., 2016). Having a particularly low or
    high capacity can affect people in many different ways, impacting daily life
    (Richmond et al., 2015).
    Low WM capacity has been linked to inattentive behavior, which can
    increase the difficulty of everyday tasks and activities requiring sustained attention (Kofler et al., 2017; Zhang et al., 2018), while STM capacity may, for
    example, explain individual differences in learning (Frensch & Miner, 1994),
    mathematical performance (Swanson & Kim, 2007), and reading comprehension (Haarmann et al., 2003). Specific courses designed to improve WM have
    been implemented using different techniques and with varying levels of success
    (Klingberg et al., 2002; for a review, see Morrison & Chein, 2011). Methods
    targeting the improvement of STM specifically have included rehearsal
    (Broadley et al., 1994), visual imagery (de la Iglesia et al., 2005), creating stories
    from the information to be remembered (McNamara & Scott, 2001), and grouping of the items into conceptual categories (Carr & Schneider, 1991).
    One promising avenue for improvement has been the introduction of mindfulness meditation (MM), gaining popularity in recent years and triggering substantial research (Keng et al., 2011). Mindfulness-based interventions have
    become common, owing to their affordability, ease of learning, and growing
    evidential support for benefits in mental health and cognitive function (Davis &
    Hayes, 2011). The term MM can describe several practices and states, with the
    main goal of reaching a state of awareness that maintains attention in the present moment (e.g., on changing sensory and mental states) while avoiding the
    intrusion of outside factors (Baltar & Filgueiras, 2018).
    Chambers et al. (2008) assessed individuals after they had attended a 10-day
    MM camp, finding that WM capacity (specifically, backward digit memory
    span) had improved. Indeed, several studies have recently demonstrated similar
    improvements on various measures of WM, including the adaptive n-back task
    and operation span, although no improvements in STM (e.g., forward digit
    span) were found (e.g., Mrazek et al., 2013; Zeidan et al., 2010). In contrast,
    Lykins et al. (2012) found no improvements in WM (letter-number sequencing;
    Wechsler, 1997) while identifying group differences (experienced meditators
    vs. controls) in measures of STM (both short delay free and cued recall;
    Delis et al., 1987).
    Youngs et al.
    1675
    While studies appear to have demonstrated relationships between MM and
    improved WM and STM capacities, the length of the MM intervention involved
    has been variable, ranging from one to eight weeks. Shorter intervention periods
    are generally preferred to longer ones due to their ease and convenience (BergenCico et al., 2013). However, there are some cases in which brief
    interventions have been shown to be marginally less effective than longer ones
    (Basso et al., 2019). In terms of implementation, there is a clear demand for
    research surrounding the effectiveness of MM in even shorter sessions, such as
    a single sitting (Chiesa et al., 2011). It is also worth noting that the motivation
    to practice MM can decrease over time and across sessions (Hafenbrack &
    Vohs, 2018). As such, the main aim of our study was to investigate whether a
    single, brief session of MM could lead to a measurable improvement in
    STM capacity.
    There are many ways to study the impact of MM on STM capacity, and the
    specific task used to quantify STM can vary. With previous research involving
    STM capacity focusing on verbal paradigms (e.g., Lykins et al., 2012), the
    effects of MM on visual STM have yet to be investigated, which is perhaps
    surprising since this can be assessed reliably and is critical for the online use
    of visual information. One method of testing visual STM is through a delayed
    nonmatching-to-sample paradigm, where participants are tasked with distinguishing between previously seen items and a novel item (e.g., using faces—
    Crook & Larrabee, 1992). As such, identification of the novel item requires
    that previously seen items are maintained in STM.
    There are many explanations as to why MM improves memory, with the
    most widely accepted explanation being through a reduction of anxiety. For
    instance, Eysenck et al. (2007) suggested that the attentional control aspect of
    the central executive is impaired by anxiety. Specifically, the inhibition function
    of the central executive can no longer effectively redirect attention away
    from task irrelevant stimuli. This, in turn, means that people find difficulty in
    completing tasks that require sustained attention as they become distracted.
    A link between attentional control and memory has been established as
    a result of this, due to the completion of memory tasks requiring effective attentional control in order to maintain attention to goal-relevant information
    (Shipstead et al., 2014).
    Researchers have suggested that attention and memory processes are closely
    related forms of cognitive control, with both likely to be influenced by MM (Jha
    et al., 2010). Furthermore, both short- and long-term memory are largely dependent on present-moment direction of attention (Cowan, 1997). Therefore, in line
    with previous work (Lykins et al., 2012), it is important to determine whether
    MM can improve performance on STM tasks. In the current study, we utilize a
    single, brief MM intervention, lasting only a few minutes. In addition, we focus
    on visual STM specifically, given the lack of previous research investigating this
    particular domain.
    1676
    Psychological Reports 124(4)
    Method
    Participants
    A total of 90 undergraduate students (age range, 18–25 years; 61 women; 83 selfreported as White) participated in exchange for course credits. Data from one
    additional participant were excluded due to a failure to complete all the tasks.
    Sample size (N ¼ 30 per group) was chosen to be comparable with previous
    research in this area (for a systematic review, see Chiesa et al., 2011).
    The university’s research ethics committee approved the experiment presented here, which was carried out in accordance with the provisions of the
    World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki. Participants provided written informed consent before taking part and were given both a written and
    verbal debriefing upon completion.
    Materials
    Participants completed two measures of mindfulness during everyday life. The
    Mindfulness Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS; Brown & Ryan, 2003) consists
    of 15 items that are rated on a 6-point Likert-type scale from 1 (almost always)
    to 6 (almost never), where the mean rating across all items represents the final
    score, with higher scores reflecting greater mindfulness. The Five-Factor
    Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ; Baer et al., 2006) consists of 39 items
    that are rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale from 1 (never or very rarely true)
    to 5 (very often or always true). Mean ratings (after reverse scoring specific
    items) are calculated for each of five facets: observing, describing, acting with
    awareness, nonjudging of inner experience, and nonreactivity to inner experience. Again, higher scores reflect greater mindfulness.
    Given that the FFMQ is derived from a factor analysis of several questionnaires including the MAAS, we would predict at least some correlation between
    participants’ scores on these instruments. However, since such relationships are
    far from perfect (Baer et al., 2006) and both measures remain popular, we
    decided to include both questionnaires in the current study.
    In the listening task, participants in the audiobook group listened to the
    beginning of “The Hobbit” (Tolkien, 2005). Those in the meditation group
    listened to a “mindfulness of body and breath” exercise (Williams & Penman,
    2011) designed to focus their attention on the movement of the breath in the
    body. We selected an audiobook for comparison since it requires a similar
    amount of attention and concentration to the meditation task, although the
    focus of attention in the two tasks was necessarily, and importantly, different.
    Both of these audio segments have featured in previous research (e.g., Kramer
    et al., 2013). Finally, for the control group, participants were simply asked to sit
    quietly and fill their time however they wished. (In the majority of cases, this
    Youngs et al.
    1677
    involved the use of personal smartphones.) The audio recordings were presented
    using closed-back headphones.
    For the face memory task, 450 images of White faces (225 women) were
    downloaded from an online database (www.facity.com), which contained
    around 2000 high-quality photographs of faces, taken front-on and with neutral
    expressions, hair pulled back, and minimal make-up. We selected only faces with
    no jewellery and who were aged approximately 18 to 40 (year of birth was
    available in the majority of cases). Images were already cropped below the
    hairline, and we additionally cropped them just below the chin, and close to
    the sides of the faces, using Adobe Photoshop CS software.
    Procedure
    Participants were tested individually in a quiet laboratory room, first completing
    both questionnaires, along with demographic information. Following this, participants performed a face memory task adapted from previous work (Crook &
    Larrabee, 1992), presented on a desktop computer using custom MATLAB
    software. On each trial, a single facial photograph was initially displayed
    onscreen, which the participant was instructed to select with the mouse. Next,
    this face and a second face appeared onscreen, and the participant was required
    to select the new face. If the correct response was given, this process would
    continue, each time introducing a new face, until a maximum of 45 faces were
    displayed (see Figure 1). Within each trial, all faces were of the same sex. Trials
    terminated when an incorrect response was given, and the number of correct
    responses was recorded. Importantly, after every response, the new display of
    faces was randomized with respect to spatial position onscreen, meaning that
    participants could not use location information to inform their decisions.
    Participants completed five trials in the first session, with the sex of faces alternating across trials. No face appeared in more than one trial.
    Upon completion, participants were randomly allocated to one of three
    groups. Those in the audiobook (11 men and 19 women) and meditation (12
    men and 18 women) groups then listened to an 8-minute audio recording with
    instructions to follow along as best they could and to inform the experimenter
    when it finished. Those in the meditation group were presented with a breathing
    exercise, while those in the audiobook group listened to a neutral recording.
    Participants in the control group (6 men and 24 women) were instructed to fill
    their time however they wished and were given 8 minutes for this task. In all
    cases, the experimenter remained in the room but did not interact with the
    participant. The assignment of participants to groups was randomized.
    After the listening/control task, participants completed the face memory task
    again (second session). The procedure was identical to earlier although only face
    images that had not appeared in the first session were presented.
    1678
    Psychological Reports 124(4)
    Figure 1. Illustration depicting the face memory task. In this example trial, the participant
    responds correctly to the first three displays. (Images not to scale.)
    Results
    Preliminary analyses
    Participants’ scores on the MAAS and FFMQ were calculated, and the associations between these measures are summarized in Table 1. As expected, we
    found some significant overlap between the two questionnaires as well as
    among the five facets of the FFMQ. Most notably, we found a large positive
    relationship between scores on the MAAS and the “acting with awareness” facet
    of the FFMQ.
    In addition, we compared participants across the three groups. We found no
    group differences in FFMQ Describing scores, FFMQ Nonjudging scores, and
    FFMQ Nonreactivity scores (in all cases, F < 2.64, p > .077, g2p < 0.06). However, we found a significant difference in MAAS scores, F(2, 87) ¼ 4.28,  corrected here and below) p ¼ .017, g2p ¼ 0.09, with post hoc tests (Dunn-Sidák revealing that participants in the control group (M ¼ 3.82, SD ¼ 0.61) showed higher scores than those in the audiobook group (M ¼ 3.38, SD ¼ 0.63; p ¼ .021). No other comparisons were significant (both ps > .087). Groups also
    differed in FFMQ Observing scores, F(2, 87) ¼ 4.15, p ¼ .019, g2p ¼ 0.09, with
    participants in the meditation group (M ¼ 3.31, SD ¼ 0.54) showing higher
    scores than those in the control group (M ¼ 2.83, SD ¼ 0.72; p ¼ .017). No
    other comparisons were significant (both ps > .200). Finally, group differences
    were also found in FFMQ Acting with Awareness scores, F(2, 87) ¼ 4.44,
    Youngs et al.
    1679
    Table 1. Correlations between questionnaire measures.
    Questionnaire
    1. MAAS
    2. FFMQ: Observing
    3. FFMQ: Describing
    4. FFMQ: Acting with awareness
    5. FFMQ: Nonjudging
    6. FFMQ: Nonreactivity
    1
    2
    3
    4
    5

    –0.05
    0.32**
    0.69***
    0.42***
    0.16

    0.20
    –0.15
    –0.30**
    0.17

    0.19
    0.19
    0.19

    0.41***
    0.08

    0.32**
    Note: MAAS: Mindful Attention Awareness Scale; FFMQ: Five-Factor Mindfulness Questionnaire.
    *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. p ¼ .015, g2p ¼ 0.09, with participants in the control group (M ¼ 3.26, SD ¼ 0.55) showing higher scores than those in the audiobook group (M ¼ 2.82, SD ¼ 0.62; p ¼ .011). No other comparisons were significant (both ps > .302).
    In two of these results, group differences suggested higher baseline mindfulness in the control than in the audiobook participants, which had no bearing on
    the hypothesized benefit of our mindfulness task over the other two groups.
    However, we also found that participants in the meditation group scored
    higher on baseline FFMQ Observing in comparison with control participants
    prior to any listening task. Perhaps reassuringly, this difference comprised only
    0.48 on a 1 to 5 scale, and we found no difference between the meditation and
    audiobook groups. As such, we could be confident that any benefit for the MM
    group during the listening task was not the result of baseline differences between
    the three groups. In addition, we included these scores as covariates in our
    analyses (see below).
    Improvements in face memory
    For each participant, we calculated the mean score across the five trials of the
    memory task for each session separately. We then calculated the difference
    between sessions (second minus first), providing us with a measure of the
    improvement due to the listening (or control) task. The difference scores were
    then analyzed as follows.
    Initial consideration of the difference scores revealed nine data points (meditation group–1; audiobook group–4; control group–4) that were classified as
    outliers (identified using boxplots produced with IBM’s SPSS Statistics software
    v25), defined as values further than 1.5 times the interquartile range from the
    nearer edge of that range. It is worth noting that four of these outliers showed
    extreme negative difference scores while the remaining five demonstrated
    extreme positive differences, suggesting no particular pattern of performance
    1680
    Psychological Reports 124(4)
    across this subsample. As a result of identifying these outliers, we decided to
    carry out two types of analyses on our data.
    Parametric analysis: One-way ANOVA
    Since both the group means and variances are sensitive to outliers, their presence
    violates the assumptions of an analysis of variance (ANOVA) by reducing the
    validity of the results. We therefore excluded the above-mentioned nine data
    points and then carried out a one-way (Group: control, audiobook, meditation)
    between-subjects ANOVA. We found a main effect of group, F(2, 78) ¼ 9.83,
    
    p < .001, g2p ¼ 0.20, with post hoc tests (again, Dunn-Sidák corrected) revealing larger difference scores for participants in the meditation group (M ¼ 2.08, SD ¼ 2.41) in comparison with those in the audiobook group (M ¼ –0.18, SD ¼ 1.79; p < .001) and the control group (M ¼ 0.18, SD ¼ 1.82; p ¼ .003). These latter two groups did not differ from each other (p ¼ .887; see Figure 2). In addition, we compared mean improvement scores to a value of zero for each group separately. For the meditation group, we found a nonzero improvement, t(28) ¼ 4.63, p < .001, Cohen’s d ¼ 0.86. In contrast, improvements did not differ from zero for both the audiobook group, t(25) ¼ 0.53, p ¼ .603, Cohen’s d ¼ 0.10, and control group, t(25) ¼ 0.52, p ¼ .609, Cohen’s d ¼ 0.10. Our preliminary analyses revealed significant differences between participants across our three groups with regard to three of their questionnaire scores: MAAS, FFMQ Observing, and FFMQ Acting with Awareness. As such, we repeated the above analysis while including these three scores as covariates. Again, we found a main effect of group, F(2, 75) ¼ 10.66, p < .001, g2p ¼ 0.22,  with post hoc tests (again, Dunn-Sidák corrected) revealing larger difference scores for participants in the meditation group in comparison with those in the audiobook group (p < .001) and the control group (p ¼ .001). These latter two groups did not differ from each other (p ¼ .998). Nonparametric analysis: Kruskal–Wallis test We also carried out a nonparametric equivalent of the above analysis, allowing us to include all data points since ranked data are far less sensitive to outliers. The Kruskal–Wallis test found a significant difference between groups, H(2) ¼ 7.74, p ¼ .022.1 Follow-up comparisons using Mann–Whitney tests2 showed larger difference ranks for participants in the meditation group in comparison with those in the audiobook group, U ¼ 285.50, z ¼ 2.43, p ¼ .015, r ¼ 0.31. In addition, those in the meditation group also showed higher difference ranks than those in the control group, U ¼ 294.50, z ¼ 2.30, p ¼ .020, r ¼ 0.30. These latter two groups did not differ from each other, U ¼ 406.50, z ¼ 0.64, p ¼ .525, r ¼ 0.08. Youngs et al. 1681 Figure 2. Mean improvement scores for the three groups. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. Discussion The goal of this research was to investigate whether a single, brief MM intervention could improve STM capacity. Our results demonstrated a significant improvement in visual STM for our MM group. In contrast, those who listened to an audiobook or filled their time however they wished failed to show an improvement. The increase in performance on the face memory task as a result of MM extends previous research demonstrating that MM can improve various components of STM (Lykins et al., 2012). That an 8-minute MM intervention was effective is an important result, building upon earlier research showing that MM sessions as brief as 10 minutes 1682 Psychological Reports 124(4) can improve attentional control and reduce psychological stress (Norris et al., 2018), two aspects considered important in WM performance (Kane et al., 2001; Schoofs et al., 2008). Our results are also consistent with research showing that time spent on a MM task may not be correlated with the strength of subsequent improvements, suggesting that the majority of MM benefits may occur early on in one’s practice (Carmody & Baer, 2009). One explanation as to why MM might improve STM capacity is that during task performance, part of STM is occupied by task-irrelevant information. MM could then improve (relevant) STM capacity by reducing this information, freeing up cognitive resources to be put to work on the task at hand. Previous research has shown that anxiety can inhibit central executive processes (Berggren et al., 2016; Park et al., 2016), and with MM reducing anxiety (Hoge et al., 2014), this could provide a potential mechanism through which MM can improve STM. In addition, MM encourages the acceptance, rather than avoidance, of thoughts and emotions as they pass through awareness. Evidence suggests that MM improves the acceptance of emotional states, resulting in greater executive control (Teper & Inzlicht, 2013; for a review, see Malinowski, 2013). Again, this may be why our MM participants showed increases in visual STM in the current experiment. However, we acknowledge that the specific mechanism through which MM affects STM has yet to be identified. Here, for practical reasons, we presented an audio recording for our MM intervention rather than a face-to-face session. Along similar lines, recent evidence has suggested that participants can benefit from completing their own MM practices via the use of a smartphone application (Walsh et al., 2019), representing the possibility that a greater proportion of the population might benefit from access to MM through solo practice. If further research supports this idea, more people might be willing to incorporate MM into their daily routines if additional costs and time constraints, typically associated with organized classes and sessions, are not required. To conclude, this study demonstrates that a single, brief MM intervention improves performance on a STM task. Our focus on visual STM, as well as the use of only minimal MM with our participants, represents important extensions to the literature with regard to measurable effects that MM has on behavioral outcomes. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Funding The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Youngs et al. 1683 ORCID iD Robin S. S. Kramer https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8339-8832 Notes 1. Monte Carlo estimate of significance based on 10,000 samples. 2. Significance values calculated using exact methods. References Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13, 27–45. Baltar, Y. C., & Filgueiras, A. (2018). The effects of mindfulness meditation on attentional control during off-season among football players. SAGE Open, 8(2). https://doi. org/10.1177/2158244018781896 Basso, J. C., McHale, A., Ende, V., Oberlin, D. J., & Suzuki, W. A. (2019). Brief, daily meditation enhances attention, memory, mood, and emotional regulation in nonexperienced meditators. Behavioural Brain Research, 356, 208–220. Bergen-Cico, D., Possemato, K., & Cheon, S. (2013). Examining the efficacy of a brief mindfulness-based stress reduction (Brief MBSR) program on psychological health. Journal of American College Health, 61(6), 348–360. Berggren, N., Curtis, H. M., & Derakshan, N. (2016). Interactions of emotion and anxiety on visual working memory performance. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(4), 1274–1281. Broadley, I., MacDonald, J., & Buckley, S. (1994). Are children with Down’s syndrome able to maintain skills learned from a short-term memory training programme? Down Syndrome Research and Practice, 2(3), 116–122. Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 822–848. Carmody, J., & Baer, R. A. (2009). How long does a mindfulness-based stress reduction program need to be? A review of class contact hours and effect sizes for psychological distress. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65(6), 627–638. Carr, M., & Schneider, W. (1991). Long-term maintenance of organizational strategies in kindergarten children. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 16(1), 61–72. Chambers, R., Lo, B. C. Y., & Allen, N. B. (2008). The impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32(3), 303–322. Chiesa, A., Calati, R., & Serretti, A. (2011). Does mindfulness training improve cognitive abilities? A systematic review of neuropsychological findings. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(3), 449–464. Cowan, N. (1997). Attention and memory: An integrated framework. Oxford University Press. Cowan, N. (2016). The many faces of working memory and short-term storage. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(4), 1158–1170. 1684 Psychological Reports 124(4) Crook, T. H., III, & Larrabee, G. J. (1992). Changes in facial recognition memory across the adult life span. Journal of Gerontology, 47(3), P138–P141. Davis, D. M., & Hayes, J. A. (2011). What are the benefits of mindfulness? A practice review of psychotherapy-related research. Psychotherapy, 48(2), 198–208. de la Iglesia, C. J. F., Buceta, J. M., & Campos, A. (2005). Prose learning in children and adults with Down syndrome: The use of visual and mental image strategies to improve recall. Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 30(4), 199–206. Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A. (1987). California verbal learning test: Adult version manual. The Psychological Corporation. Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7(2), 336–353. Frensch, P. A., & Miner, C. S. (1994). Effects of presentation rate and individual differences in short-term memory capacity on an indirect measure of serial learning. Memory & Cognition, 22(1), 95–110. Haarmann, H. J., Davelaar, E. J., & Usher, M. (2003). Individual differences in semantic short-term memory capacity and reading comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(2), 320–345. Hafenbrack, A. C., & Vohs, K. D. (2018). Mindfulness meditation impairs task motivation but not performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 147, 1–15. Hoge, E. A., Bui, E., Marques, L., Metcalf, C. A., Morris, L. K., Robinaugh, D. J., Worthington, J. J., Pollack, M. H., & Simon, N. M. (2014). Randomized controlled trial of mindfulness meditation for generalized anxiety disorder: Effects of anxiety and stress reactivity. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 74(8), 786–792. Jha, A. P., Stanley, E. A., Kiyonaga, A., Wong, L., & Gelfand, L. (2010). Examining the protective effects of mindfulness training on working memory capacity and affective experience. Emotion, 10(1), 54–64. Kane, M. J., Bleckley, M. K., Conway, A. R., & Engle, R. W. (2001). A controlledattention view of working-memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(2), 169–183. Keng, S.-L., Smoski, M. J., & Robins, C. J. (2011). Effects of mindfulness on psychological health: A review of empirical studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(6), 1041–1056. Klingberg, T., Forssberg, H., & Westerberg, H. (2002). Training of working memory in children with ADHD. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24(6), 781–791. Kofler, M. J., Sarver, D. E., Harmon, S. L., Moltisanti, A., Aduen, P. A., Soto, E. F., & Ferretti, N. (2017). Working memory and organizational skills problems in ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 59(1), 57–67. Kramer, R. S. S., Weger, U. W., & Sharma, D. (2013). The effect of mindfulness meditation on time perception. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(3), 846–852. Lykins, E. L. B., Baer, R. A., & Gottlob, L. R. (2012). Performance-based tests of attention and memory in long-term mindfulness meditators and demographically matched nonmeditators. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36(1), 103–114. Malinowski, P. (2013). Neural mechanisms of attentional control in mindfulness meditation. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7, 8. Youngs et al. 1685 McNamara, D. S., & Scott, J. L. (2001). Working memory capacity and strategy use. Memory & Cognition, 29(1), 10–17. Morrison, A. B., & Chein, J. M. (2011). Does working memory training work? The promise and challenges of enhancing cognition by training working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(1), 46–60. Mrazek, M. D., Franklin, M. S., Phillips, D. T., Baird, B., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Mindfulness training improves working memory capacity and GRE performance while reducing mind wandering. Psychological Science, 24(5), 776–781. Norris, C. J., Creem, D., Hendler, R., & Kober, H. (2018). Brief mindfulness meditation improves attention in novices: Evidence from ERPs and moderation by neuroticism. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 315.  Ortells, J. J., Noguera, C., Alvarez, D., Carmona, E., & Houghton, G. (2016). Individual differences in working memory capacity modulates semantic negative priming from single prime words. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1286. Park, J., Wood, J., Bondi, C., Del Arco, A., & Moghaddam, B. (2016). Anxiety evokes hypofrontality and disrupts rule-relevant encoding by dorsomedial prefrontal cortex neurons. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(11), 3322–3335. Richmond, L. L., Redick, T. S., & Braver, T. S. (2015). Remembering to prepare: The benefits (and costs) of high working memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(6), 1764–1777. Schoofs, D., Preuß, D., & Wolf, O. T. (2008). Psychosocial stress induces working memory impairments in an n-back paradigm. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33(5), 643–653. Shipstead, Z., Lindsey, D. R. B., Marshall, R. L., & Engle, R. W. (2014). The mechanisms of working memory capacity: Primary memory, secondary memory, and attention control. Journal of Memory and Language, 72, 116–141. Swanson, L., & Kim, K. (2007). Working memory, short-term memory, and naming speed as predictors of children’s mathematical performance. Intelligence, 35(2), 151–168. Teper, R., & Inzlicht, M. (2013). Meditation, mindfulness and executive control: The importance of emotional acceptance and brain-based performance monitoring. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(1), 85–92. Tolkien, J. R. R. (2005). The Hobbit (M. Shaw, Narr.) [Audiobook]. HarperCollins Audiobooks. Walsh, K. M., Saab, B. J., & Farb, N. A. (2019). Effects of a mindfulness meditation app on subjective well-being: Active randomized controlled trial and experience sampling study. JMIR Mental Health, 6(1), e10844. Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler adult intelligence scale: Administration and scoring manual (3rd ed.). Oxford University Press. Williams, M., & Penman, D. (2011). Mindfulness: An eight-week plan for finding peace in a frantic world (Track 1). Piatkus. Zeidan, F., Johnson, S. K., Diamond, B. J., David, Z., & Goolkasian, P. (2010). Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: Evidence of brief mental training. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(2), 597–605. Zhang, H., Chang, L., Chen, X., Ma, L., & Zhou, R. (2018). Working memory updating training improves mathematics performance in middle school students with learning difficulties. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 154. 1686 Psychological Reports 124(4) Author Biographies Molly A. Youngs is a masters student in the School of Psychology at the University of Lincoln. Samuel E. Lee was an undergraduate student at the University of Lincoln and now works in rehabilitation, using dialectical behavioural therapy and mindfulness meditation. Michael O. Mireku is a senior lecturer in the School of Psychology at the University of Lincoln. Dinkar Sharma is a reader in the School of Psychology at the University of Kent. Robin S. S. Kramer is a lecturer in the School of Psychology at the University of Lincoln. Copyright of Psychological Reports is the property of Sage Publications Inc. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

    Calculate your order
    275 words
    Total price: $0.00

    Top-quality papers guaranteed

    54

    100% original papers

    We sell only unique pieces of writing completed according to your demands.

    54

    Confidential service

    We use security encryption to keep your personal data protected.

    54

    Money-back guarantee

    We can give your money back if something goes wrong with your order.

    Enjoy the free features we offer to everyone

    1. Title page

      Get a free title page formatted according to the specifics of your particular style.

    2. Custom formatting

      Request us to use APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, or any other style for your essay.

    3. Bibliography page

      Don’t pay extra for a list of references that perfectly fits your academic needs.

    4. 24/7 support assistance

      Ask us a question anytime you need to—we don’t charge extra for supporting you!

    Calculate how much your essay costs

    Type of paper
    Academic level
    Deadline
    550 words

    How to place an order

    • Choose the number of pages, your academic level, and deadline
    • Push the orange button
    • Give instructions for your paper
    • Pay with PayPal or a credit card
    • Track the progress of your order
    • Approve and enjoy your custom paper

    Ask experts to write you a cheap essay of excellent quality

    Place an order