Article Review
Please write a brief review of the attached article and come up with three thoughtful discussion questions.
11/2/22, 4:02 PM
Discussion Leader: AS.480.600.57.FA22 Research & Writing Methods
Discussion Leader
Each week, 3 to 4 students will lead the discussion on the module’s Discussion Board by submitting
an article review post along with discussion questions.
Students will select 1 article assigned throughout the semester on the first day of class. The article
selection will be on a first-come, first-serve basis. Students who are not in class on the first day will
have an article chosen for them by the instructor. The finalized article selection can be found under
the discussion board titled Discussion Leader Sign-up
This means that starting in Module 2, students will facilitate classroom discussion (10 points)
throughout the semester. Once you have chosen your article, in the appropriate week, you will deliver
your 1 paragraph article summary and 1 paragraph analysis of the application to research and writing
principles as we’ve discussed so far in class. You will also facilitate the class discussion on the topic
by continuing to engage with the students and instructor when they respond to your questions in
class. The instructor will be on hand to help as well as to engage the class.
Discussion Leader Rubric
Points
Possible
Criteria
Article review and analysis-describes the methods or commentary
5
presented by the author and the results and conclusions are clearly
presented. Provides commentary/analysis on how the article/findings
can be used in a theoretical or real-world application (between 300-500
words)
3 Discussion questions are open-ended and enable classmates to
make a connection between this week’s topic and the article. Questions
engage the class in a discussion of the research and how it applies to
practice. Leader engages in discussion with classmates throughout the
week
5
Select Next below to begin
https://jhu.instructure.com/courses/27452/pages/discussion-leader?module_item_id=1692705
⬇︎
1/1
www.nature.com/scientificreports
OPEN
The effect of social media
on well‑being differs
from adolescent to adolescent
Ine Beyens 1*, J. Loes Pouwels
Patti M. Valkenburg 1
1
, Irene I. van Driel
1
, Loes Keijsers
2
&
The question whether social media use benefits or undermines adolescents’ well-being is an important
societal concern. Previous empirical studies have mostly established across-the-board effects among
(sub)populations of adolescents. As a result, it is still an open question whether the effects are unique
for each individual adolescent. We sampled adolescents’ experiences six times per day for one week to
quantify differences in their susceptibility to the effects of social media on their momentary affective
well-being. Rigorous analyses of 2,155 real-time assessments showed that the association between
social media use and affective well-being differs strongly across adolescents: While 44% did not
feel better or worse after passive social media use, 46% felt better, and 10% felt worse. Our results
imply that person-specific effects can no longer be ignored in research, as well as in prevention and
intervention programs.
Ever since the introduction of social media, such as Facebook and Instagram, researchers have been studying
whether the use of such media may affect adolescents’ well-being. These studies have typically reported mixed
findings, yielding either small negative, small positive, or no effects of the time spent using social media on
different indicators of well-being, such as life satisfaction and depressive symptoms (for recent reviews, see for
example1–5). Most of these studies have focused on between-person associations, examining whether adolescents
who use social media more (or less) often than their peers experience lower (or higher) levels of well-being than
these peers. While such between-person studies are valuable in their own right, several scholars6, 7 have recently
called for studies that investigate within-person associations to understand whether an increase in an adolescent’s
social media use is associated with an increase or decrease in that adolescent’s well-being. The current study
aims to respond to this call by investigating associations between social media use and well-being within single
adolescents across multiple points in t ime8–10.
Person‑specific effects. To our knowledge, four recent studies have investigated within-person associations of social media use with different indicators of adolescent well-being (i.e., life satisfaction, depression),
again with mixed r esults6, 11–13. Orben and colleagues6 found a small negative reciprocal within-person association between the time spent using social media and life satisfaction. Likewise, Boers and colleagues12 found a
small within-person association between social media use and increased depressive symptoms. Finally, Coyne
and colleagues11 and Jensen and colleagues13 did not find any evidence for within-person associations between
social media use and depression.
Earlier studies that investigated within-person associations of social media use with indicators of well-being
have all only reported average effect sizes. However, it is possible, or even plausible, that these average withinperson effects may have been small and nonsignificant because they result from sizeable heterogeneity in adolescents’ susceptibility to the effects of social media use on well-being (see14, 15). After all, an average within-person
effect size can be considered an aggregate of numerous individual within-person effect sizes that range from
highly positive to highly negative.
Some within-person studies have sought to understand adolescents’ differential susceptibility to the effects of
social media by investigating differences between subgroups. For instance, they have investigated the moderating role of sex to compare the effects of social media on boys versus g irls6, 11. However, such a group-differential
1
Amsterdam School of Communication Research, University of Amsterdam, 1001 NG Amsterdam,
The Netherlands. 2Department of Developmental Psychology, Tilburg University, 5000 LE Tilburg, The
Netherlands. *email: i.beyens@uva.nl
Scientific Reports |
(2020) 10:10763
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7
1
Vol.:(0123456789)
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
approach, in which potential differences in susceptibility are conceptualized by group-level moderators (e.g.,
gender, age) does not provide insights into more fine-grained differences at the level of the single i ndividual16.
After all, while girls and boys each represent a homogenous group in terms of sex, they may each differ on a
wide array of other factors.
As such, although worthwhile, the average within-person effects of social media on well-being obtained in
previous studies may have been small or non-significant because they are diluted across a highly heterogeneous
population (or sub-population) of adolescents14, 15. In line with the proposition of media effects theories that
each adolescent may have a unique susceptibility to the effects of social m
edia17, a viable explanation for the
small and inconsistent findings in earlier studies may be that the effect of social media differs from adolescent
to adolescent. The aim of the current study is to investigate this hypothesis and to obtain a better understanding
of adolescents’ unique susceptibility to the effects of social media on their affective well-being.
Social media and affective well‑being. Within-person studies have provided important insights into
the associations of social media use with cognitive well-being (e.g., life satisfaction6), which refers to adolescents’
cognitive judgment of how satisfied they are with their life18. However, the associations of social media use with
adolescents’ affective well-being (i.e., adolescents’ affective evaluations of their moods and emotions18) are still
unknown. In addition, while earlier within-person studies have focused on associations with trait-like conceptualizations of well-being11–13, that is, adolescents’ average well-being across specific time periods18, there is a
lack of studies that focus on well-being as a momentary affective state. Therefore, we extend previous research by
examining the association between adolescents’ social media use and their momentary affective well-being. Like
earlier experience sampling (ESM) studies among adults19, 20, we measured adolescents’ momentary affective
well-being with a single item. Adolescents’ momentary affective well-being was defined as their current feelings
of happiness, a commonly used question to measure well-being21, 22, which has high convergent validity, as evidenced by the strong correlations with the presence of positive affect and absence of negative affect.
To assess adolescents’ momentary affective well-being (henceforth referred to as well-being), we conducted
a week-long ESM study among 63 middle adolescents ages 14 and 15. Six times a day, adolescents were asked to
complete a survey using their own mobile phone, covering 42 assessments per adolescent, assessing their affective
well-being and social media use. In total, adolescents completed 2,155 assessments (83.2% average compliance).
We focused on middle adolescence, since this is the period in life characterized by most significant fluctuations in well-being23, 24. Also, in comparison to early and late adolescents, middle adolescents are more sensitive
to reactions from peers and have a strong tendency to compare themselves with others on social media and
beyond. Because middle adolescents typically use different social media platforms, in a complementary w
ay25–27,
each adolescent reported on his/her use of the three social media platforms that s/he used most frequently out of
the five most popular social media platforms among adolescents: WhatsApp, followed by Instagram, Snapchat,
YouTube, and, finally, the chat function of games28. In addition to investigating the association between overall
social media use and well-being (i.e., the summed use of adolescents’ three most frequently used platforms), we
examined the unique associations of the two most popular platforms, WhatsApp and Instagram28.
Like previous studies on social media use and well-being, we distinguished between active social media use
(i.e., “activities that facilitate direct exchanges with others”29) and passive social media use (i.e., “consuming
information without direct exchanges”29). Within-person studies among young adults have shown that passive
but not active social media use predicts decreases in well-being29. Therefore, we examined the unique associations
of adolescents’ overall active and passive social media use with their well-being, as well as active and passive use
of Instagram and WhatsApp, specifically. We investigated categorical associations, that is, whether adolescents
would feel better or worse if they had actively or passively used social media. And we investigated dose–response
associations to understand whether adolescents’ well-being would change as a function of the time they had
spent actively or passively using social media.
The hypotheses and the design, sampling and analysis plan were preregistered prior to data collection and
are available on the Open Science Framework, along with the code used in the analyses (https://osf.io/nhks2).
For details about the design of the study and analysis approach, see Methods.
Results
In more than half of all assessments (68.17%), adolescents had used social media (i.e., one or more of their three
favorite social media platforms), either in an active or passive way. Instagram (50.90%) and WhatsApp (53.52%)
were used in half of all assessments. Passive use of social media (66.21% of all assessments) was more common
than active use (50.86%), both on Instagram (48.48% vs. 20.79%) and WhatsApp (51.25% vs. 40.07%).
Strong positive between-person correlations were found between the duration of active and passive social
media use (overall: r = 0.69, p < 0.001; Instagram: r = 0.38, p < 0.01; WhatsApp: r = 0.85, p < 0.001): Adolescents
who had spent more time actively using social media than their peers, had also spent more time passively using
social media than their peers. Likewise, strong positive within-person correlations were found between the duration of active and passive social media use (overall: r = 0.63, p < 0.001; Instagram: r = 0.37, p < 0.001; WhatsApp:
r = 0.57, p < 0.001): The more time an adolescent had spent actively using social media at a certain moment, the
more time s/he had also spent passively using social media at that moment.
Table 1 displays the average number of minutes that adolescents had spent using social media in the past
hour at each assessment, and the zero-order between- and within-person correlations between the duration of
social media use and well-being. At the between-person level, the duration of active and passive social media
use was not associated with well-being: Adolescents who had spent more time actively or passively using social
media than their peers did not report significantly higher or lower levels of well-being than their peers. At the
within-person level, significant but weak positive correlations were found between the duration of active and
Scientific Reports |
Vol:.(1234567890)
(2020) 10:10763 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7
2
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
M (SD)
Well-being between-person
Well-being within-person
Well-being
5.61 (0.75)
–
Duration of overall active social media use
12.47 (11.49)
.06
–
.09**
Duration of overall passive social media use
19.71 (8.95)
.17
.07*
Duration of active Instagram use
5.15 (6.69)
− .03
.04
Duration of passive Instagram use
9.39 (4.84)
− .07
.03
Duration of active WhatsApp use
5.34 (4.14)
.08
.04
Duration of passive WhatsApp use
7.34 (3.69)
.01
.09***
Table 1. Means, standard deviations, and zero-order between-person and within-person correlations for
well-being and duration of use. Means were calculated at the between-person level and represent the average
number of minutes that adolescents had spent using social media in the past hour across assessments during
which adolescents had used social media/Instagram/WhatsApp in the past hour. All correlations are based
on the assessments during which participants had used social media/Instagram/WhatsApp, either actively or
passively. N = 63 for active and passive overall social media use; N = 60 for active and passive Instagram use;
N = 63 for active and passive WhatsApp use. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
Categorical associations
Use versus no use
(N participants = 63; N assessments = 2,155)
Dose–response associations
Duration of use
(N participants = 63; N assessments = 1,474)
Model 1A
Fixed effects
Model 2A
Fixed effects
B
(SE)
Model 1B
Random effects
p
β
B
(SE)
p
B
(SE)
Model 2B
Random effects
p
β
B
(SE)
p
Fixed part
Intercept
5.60
(.10)
< .001
7.63
5.60
(.10)
< .001
5.66
(.09)
< .001
8.05
5.66
(.09)
< .001
Assessment (WP)
.09
(.04)
.020
.09
.09
(.04)
.015
.07
(.04)
.094
.07
.06
(.04)
.091
Passive use (WP)
.14
(.09)
.111
.06
.14
(.09)
.114
.03
(.03)
.270
.04
.03
(.03)
.243
Active use (WP)
.14
(.08)
.096
.05
.15
(.08)
.058
.07
(.04)
.040
.07
.07
(.04)
.053
σ2 residual (WP)
1.16
(.11)
< .001
1.13
(.11)
< .001
1.10
(.11)
< .001
1.08
(.11)
< .001
σ2 between-person (BP)
.54
(.08)
< .001
.54
(.08)
< .001
.49
(.09)
< .001
.50
(.09)
< .001
σ2 passive use (BP)
.11
(.05)
.015
< .01
(.01)
.333
σ2 active use (BP)
.05
(.06)
.209
.01
(.01)
.221
Random part
Model fit
Deviance
6,616.58
6,603.86
4,470.03
4,467.78
AIC
6,628.58
6,619.86
4,482.03
4,483.78
BIC
6,662.63
6,665.26
4,513.80
4,526.14
Chi2 (df)
17.52 (2)
< .001
2.62 (2)
.270
Table 2. Within-person associations between adolescents’ overall use of social media and well-being. For
investigating the categorical associations, the passive and active use predictors were dummy coded (passive
use: 0 = no passive use of social media; 1 = passive use of social media; and active use: 0 = no active use of
social media; 1 = active use of social media, respectively). WP = within-person; BP = between-person. All
predictors were person-mean centered. Models for the duration of use only include assessments during which
participants had used social media, either actively or passively. p-values of the fixed part of the model are twosided, p-values of the random part of the model are one-sided.
passive overall social media use and well-being. This indicates that adolescents felt somewhat better at moments
when they had spent more time actively or passively using social media (overall), compared to moments when
they had spent less time actively or passively using social media. When looking at specific platforms, a positive
correlation was only found for passive WhatsApp use, but not for active WhatsApp use, and not for active and
passive Instagram use.
Average and person‑specific effects. The within-person associations of social media use with wellbeing and differences in these associations were tested in a series of multilevel models. We ran separate models
for overall social media use (i.e., active use and passive use of adolescents’ three favorite social media platforms,
see Table 2), Instagram use (see Table 3), and WhatsApp use (see Table 4). In a first step we examined the average categorical associations for each of these three social media uses using fixed effects models (Models 1A, 3A,
and 5A) to investigate whether, on average, adolescents would feel better or worse at moments when they had
Scientific Reports |
(2020) 10:10763 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7
3
Vol.:(0123456789)
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Categorical associations
Use versus no use
(N participants = 60; N assessments = 2,112)
Dose–response associations
Duration of use
(N participants = 60; N assessments = 1,075)
Model 3A
Fixed effects
Model 4A
Fixed effects
B
(SE)
Model 3B
Random effects
p
β
B
(SE)
p
B
(SE)
Model 4B
Random effects
p
β
B
(SE)
p
Fixed part
Intercept
5.64
(.10)
< .001
7.89
5.64
(.10)
< .001
5.69
(.10)
< .001
7.62
5.69
(.10)
< .001
Assessment (WP)
.08
(.04)
.031
.09
.08
(.04)
.033
.05
(.05)
.347
.09
.05
(.05)
.276
Passive use (WP)
.15
(.07)
.027
.14
.15
(.07)
.041
.03
(.07)
.695
.14
.06
(.07)
.440
Active use (WP)
.14
(.09)
.137
.13
.13
(.09)
.164
.05
(.06)
.428
.06
.01
(.07)
.879
σ2 residual (WP)
1.18
(.11)
< .001
1.17
(.10)
< .001
1.11
(.12)
< .001
1.07
(.12)
< .001
σ2 between-person (BP)
.54
(.08)
< .001
.54
(.08)
< .001
.47
(.08)
< .001
.47
(.08)
< .001
σ2 passive use (BP)
.02
(.05)
.335
.06
(.04)
.068
σ2 active use (BP)
.05
(.07)
.217
.04
(.04)
.142
Random part
Model fit
Deviance
6,503.54
6,501.67
3,287.59
3,279.66
AIC
6,515.54
6,517.67
3,299.59
3,295.33
BIC
6,549.48
6,562.91
Chi2 (df)
3,329.47
1.37 (2)
3,335.17
.503
19.92 (2)
< .001
Table 3. Within-person associations between adolescents’ use of Instagram and well-being. For investigating
the categorical associations, the passive and active use predictors were dummy coded (passive use: 0 = no
passive use of Instagram; 1 = passive use of Instagram; and active use: 0 = no active use of Instagram; 1 = active
use of Instagram, respectively). WP = within-person; BP = between-person. All predictors were personmean centered. Models for the duration of use only include assessments during which participants had used
Instagram, either actively or passively. p-values of the fixed part of the model are two-sided, p-values of the
random part of the model are one-sided.
Categorical associations
Use versus no use
(N participants = 63; N assessments = 2,154)
Dose–response associations
Duration of use
(N participants = 62; N assessments = 1,179)
Model 5A
Fixed effects
Model 6A
Fixed effects
B
(SE)
Intercept
5.60
Assessment (WP)
.08
Passive use (WP)
Active use (WP)
Model 5B
Random effects
p
β
B
(SE)
p
B
(SE)
(.10)
< .001
7.62
5.60
(.04)
.023
.09
.09
(.11)
< .001
5.69
(.04)
.019
.09
.16
(.05)
.001
.14
.06
(.07)
.364
.06
.22
(.09)
.022
.18
.02
(.09)
.810
− .05
σ2 residual (WP)
1.16
(.11)
σ2 between-person (BP)
.54
(.08)
< .001
1.15
(.11)
< .001
< .001
.54
(.08)
< .001
σ2 passive use (BP)
.04
(.08)
σ2 active use (BP)
.02
(.05)
Model 6B
Random effects
p
β
B
(SE)
p
(.09)
< .001
7.62
5.69
(.09)
< .001
(.04)
.049
.09
.09
(.04)
.047
(.05)
< .001
.14
.18
(.04)
< .001
(.06)
.444
.06
− .05
(.05)
.258
1.10
(.11)
< .001
1.10
(.12)
< .001
.47
(.09)
< .001
.47
(.09)
< .001
.296
< .01
(.01)
.440
.360
< .01
(.01)
.310
Fixed part
Random part
Model fit
Deviance
6,615.24
6,613.47
3,590.86
3,591.40
AIC
6,627.24
6,629.47
3,602.86
3,607.40
BIC
6,661.29
6,674.87
3,633.30
3,647.98
Chi2 (df)
1.14 (2)
.566
2.81 (2)
.245
Table 4. Within-person associations between adolescents’ use of WhatsApp and well-being. For investigating
the categorical associations, the passive and active use predictors were dummy coded (passive use: 0 = no
passive use of WhatsApp; 1 = passive use of WhatsApp; and active use: 0 = no active use of WhatsApp; 1 = active
use of WhatsApp, respectively). WP = within-person; BP = between-person. All predictors were personmean centered. Models for the duration of use only include assessments during which participants had used
WhatsApp, either actively or passively. p-values of the fixed part of the model are two-sided, p-values of the
random part of the model are one-sided.
Scientific Reports |
Vol:.(1234567890)
(2020) 10:10763 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7
4
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
used social media compared to moments when they had not (i.e., categorical predictors: active use versus no
active use, and passive use versus no passive use). In a second step, we examined heterogeneity in the withinperson categorical associations by adding random slopes to the fixed effects models (Models 1B, 3B, and 5B).
Next, we examined the average dose–response associations using fixed effects models (Models 2A, 4A, and 6A),
to investigate whether, on average, adolescents would feel better or worse when they had spent more time using
social media (i.e., continuous predictors: duration of active use and duration of passive use). Finally, we examined heterogeneity in the within-person dose–response associations by adding random slopes to the fixed effects
models (Models 2B, 4B, and 6B).
Overall social media use.. The model with the categorical predictors (see Table 2; Model 1A) showed that, on
average, there was no association between overall use and well-being: Adolescents’ well-being did not increase
or decrease at moments when they had used social media, either in a passive or active way. However, evidence
was found that the association of passive (but not active) social media use with well-being differed from adolescent to adolescent (Model 1B), with effect sizes ranging from − 0.24 to 0.68. For 44.26% of the adolescents the
association was non-existent to small (− 0.10 < r < 0.10). However, for 45.90% of the adolescents there was a weak
(0.10 < r < 0.20; 8.20%), moderate (0.20 < r < 0.30; 22.95%) or even strong positive (r ≥ 0.30; 14.75%) association
between overall passive social media use and well-being, and for almost one in ten (9.84%) adolescents there was
a weak (− 0.20 < r < − 0.10; 6.56%) or moderate negative (− 0.30 < r < − 0.20; 3.28%) association.
The model with continuous predictors (Model 2A) showed that, on average, there was a significant
dose–response association for active use. At moments when adolescents had used social media, the time they
spent actively (but not passively) using social media was positively associated with well-being: Adolescents felt
better at moments when they had spent more time sending messages, posting, or sharing something on social
media. The associations of the time spent actively and passively using social media with well-being did not differ
across adolescents (Model 2B).
Instagram use. As shown in Model 3A in Table 3, on average, there was a significant categorical association
between passive (but not active) Instagram use and well-being: Adolescents experienced an increase in wellbeing at moments when they had passively used Instagram (i.e., viewing posts/stories of others). Adolescents did
not experience an increase or decrease in well-being when they had actively used Instagram. The associations of
passive and active Instagram use with well-being did not differ across adolescents (Model 3B).
On average, no significant dose–response association was found for Instagram use (Model 4A): At moments
when adolescents had used Instagram, the time adolescents spent using Instagram (either actively or passively)
was not associated with their well-being. However, evidence was found that the association of the time spent
passively using Instagram differed from adolescent to adolescent (Model 4B), with effect sizes ranging from − 0.48
to 0.27. For most adolescents (73.91%) the association was non-existent to small (− 0.10 < r < 0.10), but for almost
one in five adolescents (17.39%) there was a weak (0.10 < r < 0.20; 10.87%) or moderate (0.20 < r < 0.30; 6.52%)
positive association, and for almost one in ten adolescents (8.70%) there was a weak (− 0.20 < r < − 0.10; 2.17%),
moderate (− 0.30 < r < − 0.20; 4.35%), or strong (r ≤ − 0.30; 2.17%) negative association. Figure 1 illustrates these
differences in the dose–response associations.
WhatsApp use. As shown in Model 5A in Table 4, just as for Instagram, we found that, on average, there was
a significant categorical association between passive (but not active) WhatsApp use and well-being: Adolescents
reported that they felt better at moments when they had passively used WhatsApp (i.e., read WhatsApp messages). For active WhatsApp use, no significant association was found. Also, in line with the results for Instagram
use, no differences were found regarding the associations of active and passive WhatsApp use (Model 5B).
In addition, a significant dose–response association was found for passive (but not active) use (Model 6A).
At moments when adolescents had used WhatsApp, we found that, on average, the time adolescents spent passively using WhatsApp was positively associated with well-being: Adolescents felt better at moments when they
had spent more time reading WhatsApp messages. The time spent actively using WhatsApp was not associated
with well-being. No differences were found in the dose–response associations of active and passive WhatsApp
use (Model 6B).
Discussion
This preregistered study investigated adolescents’ unique susceptibility to the effects of social media. We found
that the associations of passive (but not active) social media use with well-being differed substantially from
adolescent to adolescent, with effect sizes ranging from moderately negative (− 0.24) to strongly positive (0.68).
While 44.26% of adolescents did not feel better or worse if they had passively used social media, 45.90% felt
better, and a small group felt worse (9.84%). In addition, for Instagram the majority of adolescents (73.91%)
did not feel better or worse when they had spent more time viewing post or stories of others, whereas some felt
better (17.39%), and others (8.70%) felt worse.
These findings have important implications for social media effects research, and media effects research more
generally. For decades, researchers have argued that people differ in their susceptibility to the effects of media17,
leading to numerous investigations of such differential susceptibility. These investigations have typically focused
on moderators, based on variables such as sex, age, or personality. Yet, over the years, studies have shown that
such moderators appear to have little power to explain how individuals differ in their susceptibility to media
effects, probably because a group-differential approach does not account for the possibility that media users may
differ across a range of factors, that are not captured by only one (or a few) investigated moderator variables.
Scientific Reports |
(2020) 10:10763 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7
5
Vol.:(0123456789)
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Figure 1. The dose–response association between passive Instagram use (in minutes per hour) and affective
well-being for each individual adolescent (n = 46). Red lines represent significant negative within-person
associations, green lines represent significant positive within-person associations, and gray lines represent nonsignificant within-person associations. A graph was created for each participant who had completed at least 10
assessments. A total of 13 participants were excluded because they had completed less than 10 assessments of
passive Instagram use. In addition, one participant was excluded because no graph could be computed, since
this participant’s passive Instagram use was constant across assessments.
By providing insights into each individual’s unique susceptibility, the findings of this study provide an explanation as to why, up until now, most media effects research has only found small effects. We found that the
majority of adolescents do not experience any short-term changes in well-being related to their social media use.
And if they do experience any changes, these are more often positive than negative. Because only small subsets
of adolescents experience small to moderate changes in well-being, the true effects of social media reported in
previous studies have probably been diluted across heterogeneous samples of individuals that differ in their
susceptibility to media effects (also s ee30). Several scholars have noted that overall effect sizes may mask more
subtle individual differences14, 15, which may explain why previous studies have typically reported small or no
effects of social media on well-being or indicators of well-being6, 11–13. The current study seems to confirm this
assumption, by showing that while the overall effect sizes are small at best, the person-specific effect sizes vary
considerably, from tiny and small to moderate and strong.
As called upon by other scholars5, 31, we disentangled the associations of active and passive use of social
media. Research among young adults found that passive (but not active) social media use is associated with lower
levels of affective well-being29. In line with these findings, the current study shows that active and passive use
yielded different associations with adolescents’ affective well-being. Interestingly though, in contrast to previous findings among adults, our study showed that, on average, passive use of Instagram and WhatsApp seemed
to enhance rather than decrease adolescents’ well-being. This discrepancy in findings may be attributed to the
fact that different mechanisms might be involved. Verduyn and colleagues29 found that passive use of Facebook
undermines adults’ well-being by enhancing envy, which may also explain the decreases in well-being found in
our study among a small group of adolescents. Yet, adolescents who felt better by passively using Instagram and
WhatsApp, might have felt so because they experienced enjoyment. After all, adolescents often seek positive
content on social media, such as humorous posts or memes32. Also, research has shown that adolescents mainly
receive positive feedback on social m
edia33. Hence, their passive Instagram and WhatsApp use may involve the
reading of positive feedback, which may explain the increases in well-being.
Overall, the time spent passively using WhatsApp improved adolescents’ well-being. This did not differ from
adolescent to adolescent. However, the associations of the time spent passively using Instagram with well-being
did differ from adolescent to adolescent. This discrepancy suggests that not all social media uses yield personspecific effects on well-being. A possible explanation may be that adolescents’ responses to WhatsApp are more
homogenous than those to Instagram. WhatsApp is a more private platform, which is mostly used for one-toone communication with friends and a cquaintances26. Instagram, in contrast, is a more public platform, which
Scientific Reports |
Vol:.(1234567890)
(2020) 10:10763 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7
6
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
allows its users to follow a diverse set of people, ranging from best friends to singers, actors, and i nfluencers28,
and to engage in intimate communication as well as self-presentation and social comparison. Such diverse uses
could lead to more varied, or even opposing responses, such as envy versus inspiration.
Limitations and directions for future research. The current study extends our understanding of differential susceptibility to media effects, by revealing that the effect of social media use on well-being differs from
adolescent to adolescent. The findings confirm our assumption that among the great majority of adolescents,
social media use is unrelated to well-being, but that among a small subset, social media use is either related
to decreases or increases in well-being. It must be noted, however, that participants in this study felt relatively
happy, overall. Studies with more vulnerable samples, consisting of clinical samples or youth with lower socialemotional well-being may elicit different patterns of effects27. Also, the current study focused on affective wellbeing, operationalized as happiness. It is plausible that social media use relates differently with other types of
well-being, such as cognitive well-being. An important next step is to identify which adolescents are particularly
susceptible to experience declines in well-being. It is conceivable, for instance, that the few adolescents who feel
worse when they use social media are the ones who receive negative feedback on social m
edia33.
In addition, future ESM studies into the effects of social media should attempt to include one or more followup measures to improve our knowledge of the longer-term influence of social media use on affective well-being.
While a week-long ESM is very common and applied in most earlier ESM s tudies34, a week is only a snapshot of
adolescent development. Research is needed that investigates whether the associations of social media use with
adolescents’ momentary affective well-being may cumulate into long-lasting consequences. Such investigations
could help clarify whether adolescents who feel bad in the short term would experience more negative consequences in the long term, and whether adolescents who feel better would be more resistant to developing longterm negative consequences. And while most adolescents do not seem to experience any short-term increases
or decreases in well-being, more research is needed to investigate whether these adolescents may experience a
longer-term impact of social media.
While the use of different platforms may be differently associated with well-being, different types of use may
also yield different effects. Although the current study distinguished between active and passive use of social
media, future research should further differentiate between different activities. For instance, because passive use
entails many different activities, from reading private messages (e.g., WhatsApp messages, direct messages on
Instagram) to browsing a public feed (e.g., scrolling through posts on Instagram), research is needed that explores
the unique effects of passive public use and passive private use. Research that seeks to explore the nuances in
adolescents’ susceptibility as well as the nuances in their social media use may truly improve our understanding
of the effects of social media use.
Methods
Participants. Participants were recruited via a secondary school in the south of the Netherlands. Our preregistered sampling plan set a target sample size of 100 adolescents. We invited adolescents from six classrooms
to participate in the study. The final sample consisted of 63 adolescents (i.e., 42% consent rate, which is comparable to other ESM studies among adolescents; see, for instance35, 36). Informed consent was obtained from
all participants and their parents. On average, participants were 15 years old (M = 15.12 years, SD = 0.51) and
54% were girls. All participants self-identified as Dutch, and 41.3% were enrolled in the prevocational secondary education track, 25.4% in the intermediate general secondary education track, and 33.3% in the academic
preparatory education track.
Procedure. The study was approved by the Ethics Review Board of the Faculty of Social and Behavioral
Sciences at the University of Amsterdam and was performed in accordance with the guidelines formulated by
the Ethics Review Board. The study consisted of two phases: A baseline survey and a personalized week-long
experience sampling (ESM) study. In phase 1, researchers visited the school during school hours. Researchers
informed the participants of the objective and procedure of the study and assured them that their responses
would be treated confidentially. Participants were asked to sign the consent form. Next, participants completed a
15-min baseline survey. The baseline survey included questions about demographics and assessed which social
media each adolescent used most frequently, allowing to personalize the social media questions presented during the ESM study in phase 2. After completing the baseline survey, participants were provided detailed instructions about phase 2.
In phase 2, which took place two and a half weeks after the baseline survey, a 7-day ESM study was conducted, following the guidelines for ESM studies provided by van Roekel and c olleagues34. Aiming for at least
30 assessments per participant and based on an average compliance rate of 70 to 80% reported in earlier ESM
studies among adolescents34, we asked each participant to complete a total of 42 ESM surveys (i.e., six 2-min
surveys per day). Participants completed the surveys using their own mobile phone, on which the ESM software
application Ethica Data was installed during the instruction session with the researchers (phase 1). Each 2-min
survey consisted of 22 questions, which assessed adolescents’ well-being and social media use. Two open-ended
questions were added to the final survey of the day, which asked about adolescents’ most pleasant and most
unpleasant events of the day.
The ESM sampling scheme was semi-random, to allow for randomization and avoid structural patterns in
well-being, while taking into account that adolescents were not allowed to use their phone during school time.
The Ethica Data app was programmed to generate six beep notifications per day at random time points within a
fixed time interval that was tailored to the school’s schedule: before school time (1 beep), during school breaks
(2 beeps), and after school time (3 beeps). During the weekend, the beeps were generated during the morning
Scientific Reports |
(2020) 10:10763 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7
7
Vol.:(0123456789)
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
(1 beep), afternoon (3 beeps), and evening (2 beeps). To maximize compliance, a 30-min time window was
provided to complete each survey. This time window was extended to one hour for the first survey (morning)
and two hours for the final survey (evening) to account for travel time to school and time spent on evening
activities. The average compliance rate was 83.2%. A total of 2,155 ESM assessments were collected: Participants
completed an average of 34.83 surveys (SD = 4.91) on a total of 42 surveys, which is high compared to previous
ESM studies among adolescents34.
The questions of the ESM study were personalized based on the responses to the baseline survey. During the
ESM study, each participant reported on his/her use of three different social media platforms: WhatsApp and
either Instagram, Snapchat, YouTube, and/or the chat function of games (i.e., the most popular social media
platforms among a dolescents28). Questions about Instagram and WhatsApp use were only included if the participant had indicated in the baseline survey that s/he used these platforms at least once a week. If a participant
had indicated that s/he used Instagram or WhatsApp (or both) less than once a week, s/he was asked to report
on the use of Snapchat, YouTube, or the chat function of games, depending on what platform s/he used at least
once a week. In addition to Instagram and WhatsApp, questions were asked about a third platform, that was
selected based on how frequently the participant used Snapchat, YouTube, or the chat function of games (i.e.,
at least once a week). This resulted in five different combinations of three platforms: Instagram, WhatsApp, and
Snapchat (47 participants); Instagram, WhatsApp, and YouTube (11 participants); Instagram, WhatsApp, and
chatting via games (2 participants); WhatsApp, Snapchat, and YouTube (1 participant); and WhatsApp, YouTube,
and chatting via games (2 participants).
Frequency of social media use. In the baseline survey, participants were asked to indicate how often they
used and checked Instagram, WhatsApp, Snapchat, YouTube, and the chat function of games, using response
options ranging from 1 (never) to 7 (more than 12 times per day). These platforms are the five most popular platforms among Dutch 14- and 15-year-olds28. Participants’ responses were used to select the three social media
platforms that were assessed in the personalized ESM study.
Duration of social media use. In the ESM study, duration of active and passive social media use was
measured by asking participants how much time in the past hour they had spent actively and passively using
each of the three platforms that were included in the personalized ESM surveys. Response options ranged from
0 to 60 min, with 5-min intervals. To measure active Instagram use, participants indicated how much time in
the past hour they had spent (a) “posting on your feed or sharing something in your story on Instagram” and (b)
“sending direct messages/chatting on Instagram.” These two items were summed to create the variable duration
of active Instagram use. Sum scores exceeding 60 min (only 0.52% of all assessments) were recoded to 60 min.
To measure duration of passive Instagram use, participants indicated how much time in the past hour they had
spent “viewing posts/stories of others on Instagram.” To measure the use of WhatsApp, Snapchat, YouTube
and game-based chatting, we asked participants how much time they had spent “sending WhatsApp messages”
(active use) and “reading WhatsApp messages” (passive use); “sending snaps/messages or sharing something in
your story on Snapchat” (active use) and “viewing snaps/stories/messages from others on Snapchat” (passive
use); “posting YouTube clips” (active use) and “watching YouTube clips” (passive use); “sending messages via the
chat function of a game/games” (active use) and “reading messages via the chat function of a game/games” (passive use). Duration of active and passive overall social media use were created by summing the responses across
the three social media platforms for active and passive use, respectively. Sum scores exceeding 60 min (2.13% of
all assessments for active overall use; 2.90% for passive overall use) were recoded to 60 min. The duration variables were used to investigate whether the time spent actively or passively using social media was associated with
well-being (dose–response associations).
Use/no use of social media. Based on the duration variables, we created six dummy variables, one for
active and one for passive overall social media use, one for active and one for passive Instagram use, and one for
active and one for passive WhatsApp use (0 = no active use and 1 = active use, and 0 = no passive use and 1 = passive use, respectively). These dummy variables were used to investigate whether the use of social media, irrespective of the duration of use, was associated with well-being (categorical associations).
Well‑being. Consistent with previous ESM s tudies19, 20, we measured affective well-being using one item,
asking “How happy do you feel right now?” at each assessment. Adolescents indicated their response to the
question using a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (completely), with 4 (a little) as the midpoint. Convergent validity of this item was established in a separate pilot ESM study among 30 adolescents conducted by
the research team of the fourth author: The affective well-being item was strongly correlated with the presence
of positive affect and absence of negative affect (assessed by a 10-item positive and negative affect schedule for
children; PANAS-C) at both the between-person (positive affect: r = 0.88, p < 0.001; negative affect: r = − 0.62,
p < 0.001) and within-person level (positive affect: r = 0.74, p < 0.001; negative affect: r = − 0.58, p < 0.001).
Statistical analyses. Before conducting the analyses, several validation checks were performed (see34).
First, we aimed to only include participants in the analyses who had completed more than 33% of all ESM assessments (i.e., at least 14 assessments). Next, we screened participants’ responses to the open questions for unserious responses (e.g., gross comments, jokes). And finally, we inspected time series plots for patterns in answering tendencies. Since all participants completed more than 33% of all ESM assessments, and no inappropriate
responses or low-quality data patterns were detected, all participants were included in the analyses.
Scientific Reports |
Vol:.(1234567890)
(2020) 10:10763 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7
8
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Following our preregistered analysis plan, we tested the proposed associations in a series of multilevel models.
Before doing so, we tested the homoscedasticity and linearity assumptions for multilevel a nalyses37. Inspection
of standardized residual plots indicated that the data met these assumptions (plots are available on OSF at https
://osf.io/nhks2). We specified separate models for overall social media use, use of Instagram, and use of WhatsApp. To investigate to what extent adolescents’ well-being would vary depending on whether they had actively
or passively used social media/Instagram/WhatsApp or not during the past hour (categorical associations), we
tested models including the dummy variables as predictors (active use versus no active use, and passive use
versus no passive use; models 1, 3, and 5). To investigate whether, at moments when adolescents had used social
media/Instagram/WhatsApp during the past hour, their well-being would vary depending on the duration of
social media/Instagram/WhatsApp use (dose–response associations), we tested models including the duration
variables as predictors (duration of active use and duration of passive use; models 2, 4, and 6). In order to avoid
negative skew in the duration variables, we only included assessments during which adolescents had used social
media in the past hour (overall, Instagram, or WhatsApp, respectively), either actively or passively. All models
included well-being as outcome variable. Since multilevel analyses allow to include all available data for each
individual, no missing data were imputed and no data points were excluded.
We used a model building approach that involved three steps. In the first step, we estimated an intercept-only
model to assess the relative amount of between- and within-person variance in affective well-being. We estimated
a three-level model in which repeated momentary assessments (level 1) were nested within adolescents (level
2), who, in turn, were nested within classrooms (level 3). However, because the between-classroom variance in
affective well-being was small (i.e., 0.4% of the variance was explained by differences between classes), we proceeded with estimating two-level (instead of three-level) models, with repeated momentary assessments (level
1) nested within adolescents (level 2).
In the second step, we assessed the within-person associations of well-being with (a) overall active and passive
social media use (i.e., the total of the three platforms), (b) active and passive use of Instagram, and (c) active and
passive use of WhatsApp, by adding fixed effects to the model (Models 1A-6A). To facilitate the interpretation of
the associations and control for the effects of time, a covariate was added that controlled for the nth assessment
of the study week (instead of the nth assessment of the day, as preregistered). This so-called detrending is helpful
to interpret within-person associations as correlated fluctuations beyond other changes in social media use and
well-being38. In order to obtain within-person estimates, we person-mean centered all predictors38. Significance
of the fixed effects was determined using the Wald test.
In the third and final step, we assessed heterogeneity in the within-person associations by adding random
slopes to the models (Models 1B-6B). Significance of the random slopes was determined by comparing the fit
of the fixed effects model with the fit of the random effects model, by performing the Satorra-Bentler scaled
chi-square test39 and by comparing the Bayesian information criterion (BIC40) and Akaike information criterion
(AIC41) of the models. When the random effects model had a significantly better fit than the fixed effects model
(i.e., pointing at significant heterogeneity), variance components were inspected to investigate whether heterogeneity existed in the association of either active or passive use. Next, when evidence was found for significant
heterogeneity, we computed person-specific effect sizes, based on the random effect models, to investigate what
percentages of adolescents experienced better well-being, worse well-being, and no changes in well-being. In
line with Keijsers and c olleagues42 we only included participants who had completed at least 10 assessments. In
addition, for the dose–response associations, we constructed graphical representations of the person-specific
slopes, based on the person-specific effect sizes, using the xyplot function from the lattice package in R43.
Three improvements were made to our original preregistered plan. First, rather than estimating the models
with multilevel modelling in R
43, we ran the preregistered models in M
plus44. Mplus provides standardized
estimates for the fixed effects models, which offers insight into the effect sizes. This allowed us to compare the
relative strength of the associations of passive versus active use with well-being. Second, instead of using the
maximum likelihood estimator, we used the maximum likelihood estimator with robust standard errors (MLR),
which are robust to non-normality. Sensitivity tests, uploaded on OSF (https://osf.io/nhks2), indicated that the
results were almost identical across the two software packages and estimation approaches. Third, to improve the
interpretation of the results and make the scales of the duration measures of social media use and well-being
more comparable, we transformed the social media duration scores (0 to 60 min) into scales running from 0 to
6, so that an increase of 1 unit reflects 10 min of social media use. The model estimates were unaffected by this
transformation.
Reporting summary. Further information on the research design is available in the Nature Research
Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Data availability
The dataset generated and analysed during the current study is available in F
igshare45. The preregistration of the
design, sampling and analysis plan, and the analysis scripts used to analyse the data for this paper are available
online on the Open Science Framework website (https://osf.io/nhks2).
Received: 24 January 2020; Accepted: 11 June 2020
References
1. Best, P., Manktelow, R. & Taylor, B. Online communication, social media and adolescent wellbeing: A systematic narrative review.
Child Youth Serv. Rev. 41, 27–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2014.03.001 (2014).
Scientific Reports |
(2020) 10:10763 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7
9
Vol.:(0123456789)
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
2. James, C. et al. Digital life and youth well-being, social connectedness, empathy, and narcissism. Pediatrics 140, S71–S75. https://
doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-1758F(2017).
3. McCrae, N., Gettings, S. & Purssell, E. Social media and depressive symptoms in childhood and adolescence: A systematic review.
Adolesc. Res. Rev. 2, 315–330. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40894-017-0053-4 (2017).
4. Sarmiento, I. G. et al. How does social media use relate to adolescents’ internalizing symptoms? Conclusions from a systematic
narrative review. Adolesc Res Rev, 1–24, doi:10.1007/s40894-018-0095-2 (2018).
5. Orben, A. Teenagers, screens and social media: A narrative review of reviews and key studies. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-019-01825-4 (2020).
6. Orben, A., Dienlin, T. & Przybylski, A. K. Social media’s enduring effect on adolescent life satisfaction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
116, 10226–10228. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1902058116 (2019).
7. Whitlock, J. & Masur, P. K. Disentangling the association of screen time with developmental outcomes and well-being: Problems,
challenges, and opportunities. JAMA https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.3191 (2019).
8. Hamaker, E. L. In Handbook of Research Methods for Studying Daily Life (eds Mehl, M. R. & Conner, T. S.) 43–61 (Guilford Press,
New York, 2012).
9. Schmiedek, F. & Dirk, J. In The Encyclopedia of Adulthood and Aging (ed. Krauss Whitbourne, S.) 1–6 (Wiley, 2015).
10. Keijsers, L. & van Roekel, E. In Reframing Adolescent Research (eds Hendry, L. B. & Kloep, M.) (Routledge, 2018).
11. Coyne, S. M., Rogers, A. A., Zurcher, J. D., Stockdale, L. & Booth, M. Does time spent using social media impact mental health?
An eight year longitudinal study. Comput. Hum. Behav. 104, 106160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.106160 (2020).
12. Boers, E., Afzali, M. H., Newton, N. & Conrod, P. Association of screen time and depression in adolescence. JAMA 173, 853–859.
https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.1759 (2019).
13. Jensen, M., George, M. J., Russell, M. R. & Odgers, C. L. Young adolescents’ digital technology use and mental health symptoms:
Little evidence of longitudinal or daily linkages. Clin. Psychol. Sci. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702619859336 (2019).
14. Valkenburg, P. M. The limited informativeness of meta-analyses of media effects. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10, 680–682. https://doi.
org/10.1177/1745691615592237 (2015).
15. Pearce, L. J. & Field, A. P. The impact of “scary” TV and film on children’s internalizing emotions: A meta-analysis. Hum. Commun..
Res. 42, 98–121. https://doi.org/10.1111/hcre.12069 (2016).
16. Howard, M. C. & Hoffman, M. E. Variable-centered, person-centered, and person-specific approaches. Organ. Res. Methods 21,
846–876. https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428117744021 (2017).
17. Valkenburg, P. M. & Peter, J. The differential susceptibility to media effects model. J. Commun. 63, 221–243. https: //doi.org/10.1111/
jcom.12024(2013).
18. Eid, M. & Diener, E. Global judgments of subjective well-being: Situational variability and long-term stability. Soc. Indic. Res. 65,
245–277. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:SOCI.0000003801.89195.bc (2004).
19. Kross, E. et al. Facebook use predicts declines in subjective well-being in young adults. PLoS ONE 8, e69841. https: //doi.org/10.1371/
journal.pone.0069841 (2013).
20. Reissmann, A., Hauser, J., Stollberg, E., Kaunzinger, I. & Lange, K. W. The role of loneliness in emerging adults’ everyday use of
facebook—An experience sampling approach. Comput. Hum. Behav. 88, 47–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.06.011 (2018).
21. Rutledge, R. B., Skandali, N., Dayan, P. & Dolan, R. J. A computational and neural model of momentary subjective well-being.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 111, 12252–12257. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1407535111 (2014).
22. Tov, W. In Handbook of Well-being (eds Diener, E.D. et al.) (DEF Publishers, 2018).
23. Harter, S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations (Guilford Press, New York, 2012).
24. Steinberg, L. Adolescence. Vol. 9 (McGraw-Hill, 2011).
25. Rideout, V. & Fox, S. Digital Health Practices, Social Media Use, and Mental Well-being Among Teens and Young Adults in the US
(HopeLab, San Francisco, 2018).
26. Waterloo, S. F., Baumgartner, S. E., Peter, J. & Valkenburg, P. M. Norms of online expressions of emotion: Comparing Facebook,
Twitter, Instagram, and WhatsApp. New Media Soc. 20, 1813–1831. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444817707349 (2017).
27. Rideout, V. & Robb, M. B. Social Media, Social Life: Teens Reveal their Experiences (Common Sense Media, San Fransico, 2018).
28. van Driel, I. I., Pouwels, J. L., Beyens, I., Keijsers, L. & Valkenburg, P. M. ’Posting, Scrolling, Chatting & Snapping’: Youth (14–15)
and Social Media in 2019 (Center for Research on Children, Adolescents, and the Media (CcaM), Universiteit van Amsterdam,
2019).
29. Verduyn, P. et al. Passive Facebook usage undermines affective well-being: Experimental and longitudinal evidence. J. Exp. Psychol.
144, 480–488. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000057 (2015).
30. Valkenburg, P. M. & Peter, J. Five challenges for the future of media-effects research. Int. J. Commun. 7, 197–215 (2013).
31. Verduyn, P., Ybarra, O., Résibois, M., Jonides, J. & Kross, E. Do social network sites enhance or undermine subjective well-being?
A critical review. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 11, 274–302. https://doi.org/10.1111/sipr.12033 (2017).
32. Radovic, A., Gmelin, T., Stein, B. D. & Miller, E. Depressed adolescents’ positive and negative use of social media. J. Adolesc. 55,
5–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.12.002 (2017).
33. Valkenburg, P. M., Peter, J. & Schouten, A. P. Friend networking sites and their relationship to adolescents’ well-being and social
self-esteem. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 9, 584–590. https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2006.9.584 (2006).
34. van Roekel, E., Keijsers, L. & Chung, J. M. A review of current ambulatory assessment studies in adolescent samples and practical
recommendations. J. Res. Adolesc. 29, 560–577. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12471 (2019).
35. van Roekel, E., Scholte, R. H. J., Engels, R. C. M. E., Goossens, L. & Verhagen, M. Loneliness in the daily lives of adolescents: An
experience sampling study examining the effects of social contexts. J. Early Adolesc. 35, 905–930. https://doi.org/10.1177/02724
31614547049 (2015).
36. Neumann, A., van Lier, P. A. C., Frijns, T., Meeus, W. & Koot, H. M. Emotional dynamics in the development of early adolescent
psychopathology: A one-year longitudinal Study. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 39, 657–669. https: //doi.org/10.1007/s10802 -011-9509-3
(2011).
37. Hox, J., Moerbeek, M. & van de Schoot, R. Multilevel Analysis: Techniques and Applications 3rd edn. (Routledge, London, 2018).
38. Wang, L. P. & Maxwell, S. E. On disaggregating between-person and within-person effects with longitudinal data using multilevel
models. Psychol. Methods 20, 63–83. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000030 (2015).
39. Satorra, A. & Bentler, P. M. Ensuring positiveness of the scaled difference chi-square test statistic. Psychometrika 75, 243–248. https
://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-009-9135-y (2010).
40. Schwarz, G. Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann. Stat. 6, 461–464. https://doi.org/10.1214/aos/1176344136 (1978).
41. Akaike, H. A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Trans. Autom. Control 19, 716–723. https://doi.org/10.1109/
TAC.1974.1100705 (1974).
42. Keijsers, L. et al. What drives developmental change in adolescent disclosure and maternal knowledge? Heterogeneity in withinfamily processes. Dev. Psychol. 52, 2057–2070. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000220 (2016).
43. R Core Team R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, 2017).
44. Muthén, L. K. & Muthén, B. O. Mplus User’s Guide 8th edn. (Muthén & Muthén, Los Angeles, 2017).
45. Beyens, I., Pouwels, J. L., van Driel, I. I., Keijsers, L. & Valkenburg, P. M. Dataset belonging to Beyens et al. (2020). The effect of
social media on well-being differs from adolescent to adolescent. https://doi.org/10.21942/uva.12497990 (2020).
Scientific Reports |
Vol:.(1234567890)
(2020) 10:10763 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7
10
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Acknowledgements
This study was funded by the NWO Spinoza Prize and the Gravitation grant (NWO Grant 024.001.003; Consortium on Individual Development) awarded to P.M.V. by the Dutch Research Council (NWO). Additional funding
was received from the VIDI grant (NWO VIDI Grant 452.17.011) awarded to L.K. by the Dutch Research Council
(NWO). The authors would like to thank Savannah Boele (Tilburg University) for providing her pilot ESM results.
Author contributions
I.B., J.L.P., I.I.v.D., L.K., and P.M.V. designed the study; I.B., J.L.P., and I.I.v.D. collected the data; I.B., J.L.P.,
and L.K. analyzed the data; and I.B., J.L.P., I.I.v.D., L.K., and P.M.V. contributed to writing and reviewing the
manuscript.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to I.B.
Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional affiliations.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or
format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the
Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this
article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not
permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from
the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
© The Author(s) 2020
Scientific Reports |
(2020) 10:10763 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7
11
Vol.:(0123456789)
Top-quality papers guaranteed
100% original papers
We sell only unique pieces of writing completed according to your demands.
Confidential service
We use security encryption to keep your personal data protected.
Money-back guarantee
We can give your money back if something goes wrong with your order.
Enjoy the free features we offer to everyone
-
Title page
Get a free title page formatted according to the specifics of your particular style.
-
Custom formatting
Request us to use APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, or any other style for your essay.
-
Bibliography page
Don’t pay extra for a list of references that perfectly fits your academic needs.
-
24/7 support assistance
Ask us a question anytime you need to—we don’t charge extra for supporting you!
Calculate how much your essay costs
What we are popular for
- English 101
- History
- Business Studies
- Management
- Literature
- Composition
- Psychology
- Philosophy
- Marketing
- Economics