MGMT20135 Critical thinking and managerial decision-making.

Task: need to select an article from the list of articles and write a critical review report .(check the attachments and select one among the list of articles)

   The review must be 1500 words (+/-10%) in length. 

Cover page: The essay must include a cover page that contains your name, student number, resident campus, assessment title, and lecturer. 

Minimum number of peer-reviewed journal articles to pass:  10  

Both on-campus students and Distance students must write a review article and submit online via Moodle by the due date. 

Format: Text should be word-processed, with appropriate layout and use of headings/sub-headings. Times New Roman, 12 size font and line spacing (1.5). 

Referencing: The list of references should form the last page or two, at the end of the assessment. Referencing should be in a consistent APA style. 

Please save/upload your file in either a Word format ( or x) or pdf. format. 

Task Description: The purpose of this task is for you to demonstrate your ability to critically analyze, synthesize and evaluate information and present that information in a written format. A list of articles will be available on the unit’s Moodle site for you to choose from. You will then write a critical article review based around your responses to the following questions. 

Critical Analysis 

o What is the background of the research? (Background) 

o What is the research problem? What do we know about the research problem? What do we not know? (Research objective and literature review) 

o What are the core research questions the article addresses? Why are they important to address? (Research questions) 

o How was the research conducted? (Methodology) 

o What where the major findings? (Findings) 

o What are the significance of the findings for future research or management practice? (Significance/implications) 

Critical evaluation 

o In your view, are there any limitations with this research? 

o What are some of the assumptions that the authors have made in this research? Do you agree with them? 

o What is your overall evaluation of the article’s effectiveness and credibility? 

Please do not simply list the questions and your response to them. The questions are to be used simply as a means for you to critically think about your chosen article. Your review needs to contain answers to these questions, but they need to be in essay format, that is, containing introduction section, body paragraphs and a conclusion section.  

Assessment Due Date   

Week 4 Monday (30 Jul 2018) 5:00 pm AEST

You must submit your essay with a cover page on Moodle by the due date

Return Date to Students   

Week 6 Monday (20 Aug 2018)

The marks will be released with feedback by this date.

Weighting                               30%                                                              Assessment Criteria                

Introduction *3 marks    Clearly outlines the purpose, scope, structure of the essay   

Body—critical analysis *10 marks    The following list of questions are effectively addressed: o What is the background of the research? o What is the research problem? What do we know about the research problem? What do we not know? o What are the core research questions the article addresses? Why are they important to address? o How was the research conducted? o What where the major findings? o What are the significance of the findings for future research or management practice? Demonstrate ability to critically analyze, synthesize and present information in a written format 

Body—critical evaluation *10 marks    The following list of questions are effectively addressed: o In your view, are there any limitations with this research? o What are some of the assumptions that the authors have made in this research? Do you agree with them? o What is your overall evaluation of the article’s effectiveness and credibility? Demonstrate ability to critically evaluate information and present argument in a convincing manner   Conclusion *3 marks    All aspects drawn together in a brief, concise summary. Consistent with findings, no new material introduced but highlights implications or a comment on the future of the issue.   

Presentation *2 marks    High quality of expression, grammar, spelling, punctuation and proofreading. Format and layout in professional manner ( i.e. 1.5 spacing, 12-size font, Times New Roman). Assessment cover page included, within the word count.   

Referencing *2 marks    Use of APA referencing system in a consistent and correct manner in the essay itself. Inclusion of an accurate reference list on a separate page listing only the sources that actually have been used. The reference list is arranged in alphabetical order according to the authors’ last names.   

Further breakdown of each criterion (From Fail to High Distinction) is available on Moodle.

Learning and Individual Differences

2

1 (2011)

6

7–77

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Learning and Individual Differences

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/lindif

  • Epistemic beliefs and critical thinking of Chinese students
  • Ngai-Man Chan a, Irene T. Ho a,⁎, Kelly Y.L. Ku b

    a Department of Psychology, The University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam Road, Hong Kong
    b Department of Education Studies, Hong Kong Baptist University, Kowloon Tong, Hong Kong

    ⁎ Corresponding author. Tel.: +8

    5

    2 2859 2296; fax:
    E-mail addresses: h015

    3

    802@graduate.hku.hk (N.-M

    kellyku@hkbu.edu.hk (K.Y.L. Ku).

    10

    4

    1-6080/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier Inc. Al
    doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2010.11.001

    a b s t r a c t

    a r t i c l e i n f o

    Article history:
    Received 18 October 2009
    Received in revised form 23 August 2010
    Accepted 3 November 2010

    Keywords:

    Epistemic belief

    Critical thinking
    Chinese student
    Epistemology
    Individual difference

    Two studies were carried out to examine the relationship between epistemic beliefs and critical thinking. In
    the first study, 138 Chinese undergraduates completed the adapted Epistemic Beliefs Inventory (EBI) and the
    Halpern Critical Thinking Assessment Using Everyday Situations. Their cognitive ability and thinking
    dispositions were also measured. Results showed that other than cognitive ability, the belief that knowledge is
    certain was most related to thinking performance. In the second study, 111 undergraduates read a passage on
    a controversial issue and were asked to indicate their position, explain the reasoning behind, and rate their
    confidence in judgment. They were then presented relevant counterarguments and asked to respond to them.
    Participants were also administered the adapted EBI. There was clear evidence that those believing
    knowledge to be certain exhibited poorer two-sided thinking and a stronger tendency to devaluate or ignore
    counterarguments. Implications for the enhancement of critical thinking are discussed.

    +852 2858 3518.
    . Chan), itfho@hku.hk (I.T. Ho),

    l rights reserved.

    © 2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

    1. Introduction

    With the advent of the Information Age and increased complexity
    of society, critical thinking has been receiving attention as an ability
    that enables one to deal with myriad information, to make reasonable
    judgments, and to participate in society as competent citizens
    (Halpern, 2003; Paul, 1984). Schools have been given the important
    responsibility of helping students learn to think deeply and rationally
    so that they can evaluate different arguments and views effectively
    (Brown, 1997). How critical thinking may be fostered is a question
    that educators and psychologists have been asking.

    Among the various factors affecting critical thinking performance,
    personal epistemology, defined as one’s views about the nature of
    knowledge and knowing, has been proposed to be relevant (Brabeck,
    1983; Gallagher, 1998; Jones, Merritt, & Palmer, 1999; Kuhn, 1999;
    Kurfiss, 1988; Mines, King, Hood, & Wood, 1990; Schraw, 2001). A
    better understanding of how personal epistemology affects thinking
    performance would help improve related instructional practices.

    1.1. Epistemic beliefs

    Personal epistemology refers to “beliefs about the definition of
    knowledge, how knowledge is constructed, how knowledge is
    evaluated, where knowledge resides, and how knowing occurs”
    (Hofer, 2002, p. 4). As these beliefs influence how people handle and
    use knowledge, they play an important role in students’ thinking,

    learning, and motivation (Hofer, 2001; Hofer & Printrich, 1997; Muis,
    2007; Schommer, 1994; Schraw & Sinatra, 2004).

    Researchers have differed in their approaches to the study of
    personal epistemology. One approach, initiated by Schommer (1990,
    1994), has conceptualized personal epistemology as a system of more or
    less independent beliefs. A five-factor model of epistemological beliefs
    has been proposed, consisting of beliefs about simple knowledge
    (knowledge as an accumulation of discrete and unambiguous facts),
    certain knowledge (knowledge is absolute and unchanging), omniscient
    authority (authorities have best access to knowledge), quick learning
    (learning occurs quickly or not at all), and innate ability (the ability to
    acquire knowledge is fixed).

    The measurement of these beliefs started with Schommer’s
    Epistemological Beliefs Questionnaire (Schommer, 1990), which
    utilizes a Likert-type scale to assess the strength of various epistemic
    beliefs. This instrument spurred efforts by other researchers to
    develop similar measures for the study of epistemic beliefs (e.g.,
    Bendixen, Schraw, & Dunkle, 1998; Jehng, Johnson, & Anderson,
    1993; Wood & Kardash, 2002). One example was the development of
    the 32-item Epistemic Belief Inventory (EBI) by Bendixen and
    colleagues (Bendixen et al., 1998). The EBI was modeled on Schommer’s
    questionnaire but was found to have better predictive validity and test

    retest reliability (Schraw, Bendixen, & Dunkle, 2002).

    Quantitative instruments of this kind allow us to differentiate
    individuals on the level of sophistication or maturity in their handling
    of knowledge during thinking and learning. Based on the five-
    dimension conceptualization, sophisticated beliefs embrace “evolving
    knowledge, multiple approaches to the justification of knowledge,
    integration of knowledge,…, gradual learning, and ever growing
    ability to learn” (Schommer-Aikins, 2002, p.113). For example, if a

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.11.001

    mailto:h0153802@graduate.hku.hk

    mailto:itfho@hku.hk

    mailto:kellyku@hkbu.edu.hk

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.11.001

    http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/10416080

    68 N.-M. Chan et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 21 (2011) 67–77

    student tends to believe that knowledge is absolute and knowable
    with certainty, we refer to him or her as having a naïve belief in
    knowledge being certain. In contrast, a student who appreciates the
    tentative nature of knowledge would be considered having a more
    sophisticated or mature belief in knowledge being uncertain. More
    sophisticated beliefs in the uncertain, relative, and complex nature of
    knowledge are associated with the ability and readiness to take in
    new ideas or change old ones, therefore supporting flexible thinking.

    Closely associated with the development of epistemic belief
    measures have been efforts to delineate more clearly the multi-
    dimensional nature of these beliefs. While some researchers have
    found evidence for Schommer’s original five-dimensional theory of
    epistemic beliefs (Schraw et al., 2002), others have been unable to
    replicate the proposed dimensions at the item level. For example,
    Schommer herself did not generate a clear omniscient authority
    dimension in her own study (1990), while Nussbaum and Bendixen
    (2003) found that omniscient authority and quick learning failed to
    emerge as distinct factors.

    Related studies in non-Western contexts have also suggested
    cross-cultural differences in the structure, level, and effects of
    epistemic beliefs (Khine, 2008). For example, researchers studying
    Chinese populations often highlight the emphasis on respect for
    authority and conformity in the Confucian culture in their prediction
    or interpretation of related results (Chan & Elliot, 2004a; Lin, 2001;
    Qian & Pan, 2002). However, findings have been inconsistent and so
    far no clear patterns of the structure and effects of Chinese people’s
    epistemic beliefs have emerged. As these studies are still very limited
    in number and have generated diverse findings, more investigations
    in relation to a wider range of learning and thinking processes are
    needed (for reviews of related issues see Chan & Elliot, 2004a; Duell &
    Schommer-Aikins, 2001; Hofer, 2008). The accumulation of empirical
    findings would help generate clearer patterns of universal and
    culture-specific aspects of epistemic beliefs and their effects.

    1.2. Epistemic beliefs and critical thinking

    Critical thinking is “reasonable reflective thinking that is focused
    on deciding what to believe and do” (Ennis, 1991, p.7). It involves an
    active evaluation of one’s thinking process, which would provide
    useful and accurate feedbacks to improve the thinking (Halpern, 1998,
    1999). Although authors vary in the breadth of their definition of
    critical thinking, overall there is agreement about judgment, reasoning,
    meta-cognition, and reflective thinking being the major components
    (see review by Fischer & Spiker, 2000). For example, Halpern (1998)
    proposed a taxonomy of five major critical thinking skills comprising
    verbal reasoning, argument analysis, hypothesis testing, assessing
    likelihood, and decision-making/problem-solving. These are skills
    frequently used in a wide range of everyday evaluative thinking.

    In situations requiring critical thinking, the adequacy of individuals’
    epistemological theories will in some way determine how they
    approach the task and what meaning they make of the given
    information. As illustrated above, sophisticated beliefs underlie flexible
    thinking, which is essential in the process of thinking critically. Indeed,
    many researchers share the notion that advanced epistemological
    awareness is a prerequisite for one to engage in critical thinking
    (Gallagher, 1998; Jones et al., 1999; Kurfiss, 1988), with related
    empirical evidence available. For example, Brabeck (1983) found a
    significant positive correlation between epistemological understanding
    and critical thinking, whereas Mines and colleagues (Mines et al., 1990)
    noted that “students who reason using the assumptions of the higher
    stages of reflective judgment demonstrate better critical thinking skills
    than do those who use lower stage assumptions” (p. 545). Other
    researchers also explicitly assert that personal epistemology has the
    potential to inform educators about ways to enhance students’ thinking
    (Kuhn, 1999; Schraw, 2001).

    1.3. Effects of epistemic beliefs on argumentation

    While researchers have argued for a relationship between
    epistemic beliefs and critical thinking performance, these beliefs
    have been particularly observed to produce impacts on argumenta-
    tion skills and argument analysis, which constitute a specific and
    salient aspect of critical thinking ability (see for example, Halpern’s
    1998 taxonomy of critical thinking skills). Indeed, of the many studies
    linking epistemic beliefs to thinking performance, most examine this
    relationship in contexts that involve the evaluation of two-sided
    arguments or multiple solutions in relation to controversial or
    ambiguous issues.

    Oneimportant observationfrom these studies is that naïve epistemic
    beliefs would induce simplistic approaches to complex issues, often
    manifested in an inadequate consideration of opposing views. For
    example, it was shown that individuals adopting naïve epistemic beliefs
    tended to draw oversimplified or absolute conclusions after reading
    passages highlighting two opposing views on controversial issues in the
    psychology and nutrition domains (Schommer, 1990). They would also
    write conclusions that failed to reflect the inconclusive nature of mixed
    evidence when considering controversial issues such as HIV–AIDS
    relationship, abortion, and communication styles of men and women
    (Kardash & Scholes, 1996; Schommer-Aikins & Hutter, 2002).

    Furthermore, it has been observed that people with naïve beliefs
    would treat information that does not support their existing beliefs in a
    biased manner. For example, they would use fewer cognitive strategies
    when reading dual-position text, reflecting a tendency to ignore certain
    information, and distort contradictory information in order to make it
    consistent with preexisting beliefs during free recall of text information
    (Kardash & Howell, 2000). In another study, naïve epistemic beliefs
    were found to be associated with the tendency to decline scientific
    explanation when learning about the potentially controversial topic of
    human evolution (Sinatra, Southerland, McConaughy, & Demastes,
    2003). Also the conceptual change of people holding naïve epistemic
    beliefs was found to be much more limited after reading refutation text
    that directly confronted their misconceptions about a physics topic
    (Qian & Alvermann, 1995).

    Generally, people with naïve epistemic beliefs have been found to
    avoid argumentative situations (Nussbaum & Bendixen, 2003) and
    perform poorly in ill-defined tasks with multiple, non-guaranteed
    solutions (Schraw, Dunkle, & Bendixen, 1995). In other words, the
    level of sophistication of an individual’s epistemic beliefs is particu-
    larly associated with performance on open-ended and ill-structured
    thinking tasks which involve comparing alternative viewpoints, the
    strength of evidence, and the adequacy of arguments in order to arrive
    at a judgment. It is in these situations that an absolutist tendency
    appears to produce the greatest impact on thinking and information
    processing.

    1.4. Epistemic beliefs and other critical thinking factors

    Researchers have largely conceptualized critical thinking in terms
    of two major factors: cognitive skills or ability and thinking
    dispositions. There is general consensus that although some people
    may have excellent cognitive abilities which are necessary for
    thinking critically, they may not actively or effectively engage in
    such thinking if they lack the disposition to do so (Ennis, 1991;
    Facione, Facione, & Giancarlo, 2000; Halpern, 1998; Perkins, Jay, &
    Tishman, 1993). If epistemic beliefs have also been found to be related
    to critical thinking performance, it would be necessary to investigate
    how these beliefs relate to ability and dispositions and to examine
    their relative effects on thinking performance.

    Cognitive ability and epistemic beliefs are likely to have reciprocal
    influences on each other. On the one hand, people with limited
    cognitive ability may find it difficult to process information from
    multiple perspectives, as in the comparison of arguments, therefore

    69N.-M. Chan et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 21 (2011) 67–77

    discouraging them from endorsing more sophisticated epistemic
    beliefs that acknowledge the tentative and complex nature of
    knowledge. On the other hand, persons with an absolutist inclination
    or naïve personal epistemology, with their belief in knowledge being
    facts that are either right or wrong, may not see the need to engage in
    complex intellectual activities such as argument (Hofer & Printrich,
    1997; Kuhn, 1999, 2001; Weinstock & Cronin, 2003), thus limiting
    their practice and development of higher level reasoning skills.

    Thinking dispositions that have been suggested to facilitate critical
    thinking include having an interest in thinking (e.g., need for
    cognition and inquisitiveness), a mature approach to making judg-
    ments (e.g., openness and truth-seeking), and being careful and
    systematic in thinking (e.g., conscientiousness and systematicity)
    (American Philosophical Association, 1990; Ennis, 1991; Facione et al.,
    2000; Perkins et al., 1993). More advanced development in episte-
    mological understanding not only fosters thinking skills but also
    enhances the value for critical thinking. As individuals with more
    sophisticated epistemic beliefs see the need to evaluate assertions
    based on criteria of argument and evidence, they would be more
    willing to engage in effortful thinking and be open-minded about new
    evidence or alternatives. In return, these thinking inclinations will
    also fortify sophisticated epistemic beliefs, as the habit of engaging in
    complex reasoning would reinforce the experience of knowledge
    being complex and tentative.

    Thus, cognitive abilities, thinking dispositions and epistemic
    beliefs probably develop in synchrony. Given that these individual
    difference variables are likely to be correlated, the extent to which
    personal epistemology provides unique contribution to critical
    thinking on top of the contribution of cognitive abilities and thinking
    dispositions requires investigation.

    1.5. The present research

    Current evidence as reviewed above has pointed to the phenom-
    enon that individuals with more sophisticated epistemic beliefs tend
    to be better critical thinkers. However, more research is needed to
    gain a better picture of how naïve beliefs about knowledge hinder
    good thinking. In particular, there has not been research with a focus
    on the simultaneous effects of epistemic beliefs, cognitive ability, and
    thinking dispositions on critical thinking. Furthermore, extending
    related research beyond Western contexts would throw light on the
    extent to which the hypothesized relationships are universally
    applicable.

    The present research, consisting of two studies, aimed at extending
    the empirical basis for the epistemic belief-critical thinking relation-
    ship by examining it in groups of Chinese students in Hong Kong. The
    first study was an examination of general relationships that constitute
    basic understanding, including: (i) What is the factor structure of
    epistemic beliefs among Chinese students and which aspect(s) of
    these beliefs produce most salient effects on critical thinking?
    (ii) What is the pattern of relationships between epistemic beliefs
    and cognitive ability as well as thinking dispositions? (iii) What are
    the unique and relative contributions of beliefs, ability, and disposi-
    tions to critical thinking performance? Study 2 was a more focused
    investigation of how epistemic beliefs affect argumentation, which is
    an essential aspect of critical thinking.

    2. Study 1

    There were two focuses of examination in Study 1. Firstly, we
    explored the factor structure of epistemic beliefs as well as the
    relationships between these beliefs and other critical thinking factors,
    namely cognitive ability and thinking dispositions. Secondly, we
    examined whether epistemic beliefs had a unique contribution to
    students’ critical thinking performance beyond their effects through
    associations with cognitive ability and thinking dispositions. Based on

    previous findings from Western research, the following hypotheses
    were tested:

    (i) More sophisticated epistemic beliefs are associated with better
    cognitive ability.

    (ii) More sophisticated epistemic beliefs are associated with higher
    levels of desirable thinking dispositions.

    (iii) More sophisticated epistemic beliefs are associated with better
    critical thinking performance.

    (iv) Epistemic beliefs have unique effects on critical thinking
    performance over and above those contributed by cognitive
    ability and thinking dispositions.

    2.1. Method

    2.1.1. Participants
    Participants were138 Chinese undergraduate students (40 males

    and 98 females) at a comprehensive university in Hong Kong,
    recruited through campus-wide advertising. Ages ranged from 19 to
    32 (M=20.72, SD=1.60) and their average GPA was 2.92 (SD=.51).
    Participants consisted of students in their first (47.8%), second (37.0%)
    and final (15.2%) years of study with major subjects in social science,
    arts, science, business, and others. Participation was on a voluntary
    and anonymous basis.

    2.1.2. Measures
    A pilot study was carried out to examine the applicability of the

    Epistemic Beliefs Inventory (EBI) (Bendixen et al., 1998) for use with
    the present participants, in particular its reliability and factorial
    structure. The Inventory consists of 32 items falling under five factors,
    namely Certain Knowledge, Simple Knowledge, Innate Ability, Quick
    Learning and Omniscient Authority. The Inventory was found to have
    better test–retest reliability (stability coefficients ranging from .62 to
    .81) and predictive validity than earlier instruments measuring
    similar constructs (Schraw et al., 2002).

    In the pilot study, the Chinese version of the 32 items was derived
    through a translation–back-translation procedure. Then 366 univer-
    sity and high school students in Hong Kong were asked to rate the
    items on five-point Likert scales (“Strongly agree” to “Strongly
    disagree”). Results from exploratory factor analysis showed that
    consistent with the findings from an earlier study by Nussbaum and
    Bendixen (2003), the Quick Learning and Omniscient Authority
    dimensions did not emerge as distinct factors. Instead, three
    interpretable and distinct factors with satisfactory internal consisten-
    cies (Cronbach’s alphas above .65) resulted, namely Innate Ability,
    Simple Knowledge and Certain Knowledge. Based on the results from
    this pilot study, three scales, each with four items (a total of 12 items)
    selected from the EBI, were used in the present study. Examples of
    items are: “Smart people are born that way” (Innate Ability), “If a
    person tries too hard to understand a problem, they will most likely
    end up confused” (Simple Knowledge), and “Absolute moral truth
    does not exist” (Certain Knowledge; reverse coded). Higher scores on
    these scales indicate more naïve beliefs.

    The Halpern Critical Thinking Assessment Using Everyday Situations
    (HCTAES; Halpern, 2007) was used to measure participants’ critical
    thinking performance. The HCTAES consists of 25 everyday scenarios,
    each followed by open-ended and multiple-choice questions. The test
    provides an overall index of critical thinking performance covering all
    major critical thinking skills: verbal reasoning, argument analysis,
    hypothesis testing, assessing likelihood and decision-making/problem-
    solving. The following is a hypothetical item similar in length and
    presentation to those in the HCTAES:

    Results from a recent study indicated that female adolescents who
    perceive themselves as being unpopular among peers are more likely to
    be overweight. The researchers suggested that running social skills

    Table 1
    Factor loadings of epistemic belief items and factor correlations in Study 1 (N=138).

    Item Innate
    ability

    Certain
    knowledge

    Simple
    knowledge

    1. Smart people are born that way .85
    2. People can’t do too much about how
    smart they are

    .62

    3. Some people will never be smart no
    matter how hard they work

    .51

    4. How well you do in school depends
    on how smart you are

    .40

    5. Absolute moral truth does not exista .73
    6. Truth means different things to

    different peoplea

    .59

    7. Sometimes there are no right answers
    to life’s problemsa

    .59

    8. What is true today will be true tomorrow .40
    9. Too many theories just complicate things .84
    10. The best ideas are often the most simple .64
    11. If a person tries too hard to understand a

    problem, they will most likely end up
    confused

    .53

    12. Things are simpler than most professors
    would have you believe

    .46

    Factor correlations
    Innate ability 1
    Certain knowledge −.09 1
    Simple knowledge .08 .05 1

    a Item reversed coded.

    70 N.-M. Chan et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 21 (2011) 67–77

    training programs for female adolescents who are overweight would help
    solve their weight problems.

    Open-ended question:
    Based on this information, would you support this idea as a way of

    solving overweight problems for female adolescents? Type “yes” or “no”
    and explain why or why not.

    Forced-choice question:
    Based on this information, which of the following is the best answer?
    (Four choices provided).
    Sample choice: Social skills training will probably reduce overweight

    problems among female adolescents because the researchers found that
    girls who perceive themselves as being unpopular among peers are more
    likely to be overweight.

    The HCTAES have been used with different samples of American
    students with convergent and divergent validity evidence reported,
    including high positive correlations (in the .50 to .70 range) with a
    number of achievement or ability tests (e.g., SAT-Verbal, SAT-Math
    and GRE-Analytic), and moderate correlations in the .30 to .40 range
    with measures of need for cognition and conscientiousness. Reliability
    coefficients (Cronbach’s alphas) of over .80 have been reported for
    different groups of students (Halpern, 2007). Preliminary studies on
    the appropriateness of the test for use with Chinese students were
    conducted (Hau et al., 2006). Analyses of the results from these
    studies helped to establish the validity and reliability of the Chinese
    version of the test, with Cronbach’s alphas for different samples of
    secondary and university students ranging from .65 to .74. All items
    were scored according to the standards provided in the original test
    (Halpern, 2007) and a total score was derived by summing all the item
    scores.

    The Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI) of the Chinese version of the
    Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Third Edition (WAIS-III, Chinese
    Edition, 2002) was used to measure participants’ cognitive–verbal
    ability. The VCI is derived from scores on the Vocabulary, Similarities
    and Information subtests of the WAIS-III. Previous studies have shown
    verbal abilities to be the strongest cognitive predictor of critical
    thinking performance (Clifford, Boufal, & Kurtz, 2004).

    Three dispositional variables that are considered important
    personal traits that contribute to critical thinking and are commonly
    examined in related studies (e.g., Clifford et al., 2004; Halpern, 2007;
    Sá, West, & Stanovich, 1999; Spector, Schneider, Vance, & Hezlett,
    2000; Toplak & Stanovich, 2002; West, Toplak, & Stanovich, 2008)
    were measured. These included need for cognition, open-mindedness,
    and conscientiousness. All items of the scales were rated on five-point
    Likert scales, ranging from “Strongly agree” to “Strongly disagree”. The
    Chinese version of the Need for Cognition Scale-Short Form (NCS-SF;
    Cacioppo, Petty, & Kao, 1984; Hui, 2003) measures the tendency to
    engage in and enjoy effortful thinking. It consists of 18 items and a
    sample item is “The notion of thinking abstractly is appealing to me”.
    This scale was found to be valid and reliable for use with Chinese
    students in previous studies (Kao, 1994; Ku & Ho, 2010; Kuang, Shi,
    Cai, & Wang, 2005). The Openness to Experience Subscale and the
    Conscientiousness Subscale of the NEO Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI,
    Costa & McCrae, 1992) were used as measures of open-mindedness
    and conscientiousness. They each consist of 12 items. Sample items
    include “I believe controversial topics will only confuse students”
    (reverse coded) for openness and “I work towards my goal
    systematically” for conscientiousness. The Chinese version of the
    NEO-FFI was found to have good reliability for use with Chinese
    students in previous studies (e.g., Ku & Ho, 2010; Zhang, 2002).

    2.1.3. Procedure
    Participants were tested in two sessions. In the first session, they

    completed the HCTAES. Within a week, participants came back for the
    second session, where they completed the various self-report scales
    and were individually administered the WAIS-III subtests by school
    psychology trainees who had received training on administering the

    tests. Background information on participants’ gender, age, GPA, and
    socio-economic status (SES – family income, fathers’ education,
    mothers’ education) was also gathered.

    2.2. Results and discussion

    2.2.1. Preliminary analyses
    The factorial structure of the adapted three-dimensional EBI was

    further examined by confirmatory factor analysis with the data from this
    group of students. The fit of the data to the a priori three-factor model
    was reasonably good as indicated by the various goodness of fit indexes;
    χ2(51)=71.28, RMSEA=.051, NNFI=.92, CFI=.94, with all standard-
    ized factor loadings greater than .40 (see Table 1). Internal consistency
    as measured by Cronbach’s alphas for the Innate Ability Scale, Certain
    Knowledge Scale and Simple Knowledge Scale were .67, .66, and .71
    respectively. The inter-factor correlations were all less than .10,
    suggesting that the three were quite distinct belief dimensions.

    Internal consistency coefficients for the other scales as measured
    by Cronbach’s alphas were .69 for the HCTAES, .87 for the NCS-SF, .67
    for the Openness to Experience Scale, and .76 for the Conscientious-
    ness Scale. Means and standard deviations of the variables are given in
    Table 2. Zero-order correlations among the variables are given in
    Table 3.

    2.2.2. Relations between epistemic beliefs and other critical
    thinking factors

    To examine how students’ epistemic beliefs were related to
    demographic variables (gender, age, SES composite derived from
    family income, father’s education and mother’s education), cognitive
    ability and dispositions, a set of regression analyses with each of the
    beliefs as criterion variables were conducted (Table 4). Results
    showed that being younger in age and a lower need for cognition
    would significantly predict a stronger belief in ability being innate.
    Being male, a lower verbal ability and being less open to experience
    predicted a higher tendency to believe in knowledge being certain.
    None of the variables examined were related to the belief in
    knowledge being simple. Among the three epistemic beliefs, Certain

    Table 2
    Means, standard deviations, and ranges of scores (Studies 1 and 2a).

    Variable Mean SD Potential
    range

    Actual
    range

    Study 1
    Cognitive ability (VCI) 105.47 7.91 Mean=100 61–131
    Openness (NEO-FFI-O) 42.20 5.39 12–60 29–56
    Conscientiousness (NEO-FFI-C) 41.71 5.69 12–60 25–53
    Need for cognition (NCS-SF) 58.56 9.52 18–90 37–81
    Epistemic beliefs (EBI)

    Innate ability 12.77 3.04 4–20 4–19
    Certain knowledge 8.41 2.82 4–20 4–17
    Simple knowledge 12.83 2.80 4–20 4–19

    Critical thinking (HCTAES) 115.79 13.06 0–194 73–147

    Study 2
    Epistemic beliefs (EBI)

    Innate ability 11.57 2.72 4–20 5–19
    Certain knowledge 8.77 2.53 4–20 4–18
    Simple knowledge 13.37 2.83 4–20 7–19

    Two-sided argument skills

    4.25 1.54 1–6 1–6
    Confidence 2.52 .82 1–5 1–4
    Convincingness of counterargument 3.19 .84 1–5 1–4
    Tackling of counterargument 1.92 .78 1–3 1–3

    Note. VCI=Verbal Comprehension Index of WAIS-III. NEO-FFI-O=NEO-FFI, Openness
    to Experience Subscale. NEO-FFI-C=NEO-FFI, Conscientiousness Subscale. NCS-
    SF=Need For Cognition Scale—Short Form. EBI=Epistemic Beliefs Inventory.
    HCTAES=Halpern Critical Thinking Assessment Using Everyday Situations.

    a NStudy 1=138, NStudy 2=111.

    Table 4
    Relations between epistemic beliefs and other critical thinking factors (Study 1,
    N=138).

    Criterion variable (standardized beta)

    Epistemic belief

    Predictor Innate
    ability

    Certain
    knowledge
    Simple
    knowledge

    Demographics
    Gender −.02 −.30*** −.10
    Age −.25** .10 −

    .12

    SES .08 .01 .07

    Cognitive ability
    GPA −.17 .13 .03
    VCI .09 −.18* .03

    Thinking dispositions
    Openness (NEO-FFI-O) .21 −.38*** −.20
    Conscientiousness (NEO-FFI-C) .10 −.13 −.10
    Need for cognition (NCS-SF) −.38** .07 −.14

    Multiple R2 .15 .27 .13

    Note. VCI=Verbal Comprehension Index of WAIS-III. NEO-FFI-O=NEO-FFI, Openness
    to Experience Subscale. NEO-FFI-C=NEO-FFI, Conscientiousness Subscale. NCS-
    SF=Need For Cognition Scale—Short Form.
    *pb .05, **pb .01, ***pb .001.

    71N.-M. Chan et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 21 (2011) 67–77

    Knowledge appeared to have the strongest relationship with
    thinking-related constructs. Cognitive ability and dispositions, to-
    gether with gender, accounted for 27% of the variance in this belief.

    2.2.3. Epistemic beliefs as predictors of critical thinking performance
    Stepwise regressions were conducted to examine the effects of

    various variables on critical thinking. Demographic variables were
    entered in the first step, followed by cognitive ability indexes,
    dispositional factors, and finally epistemic beliefs. The results
    (Table 5) indicated that neither demographics nor dispositional factors
    significantly predicted thinking performance. Rather, cognitive–verbal
    ability (VCI) was the most significant predictor. Moreover, the naïve
    beliefs that ability is innate, and more importantly that knowledge is
    certain, predicted poorer critical thinking, after controlling for their

    Table 3
    Zero-order correlations among variables (Study 1, N=138).

    1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

    1. Cognitive ability
    (VCI)

    2. Openness
    (NEO-FFI-O)

    .17* –

    3. Conscientiousness
    (NEO-FFI-C)

    −.02 −.10 –

    4. Need for cognition
    (NCS-SF)

    .15 .55** .14 –

    5. EBI-Innate ability .10 −.02 −.05 −.20* –
    6. EBI-Certain

    knowledge
    −.21* −.34** .03 −.15 −.09 –

    7. EBI-Simple
    knowledge

    −.02 −.27** −.11 −.26** .07 .04 –

    8. Critical thinking
    (HCTAES)

    .46** .12 −.07 .17 −.13 −.29** −.12 –

    Note. VCI=Verbal Comprehension Index of WAIS-III. NEO-FFI-O=NEO-FFI, Openness to
    Experience Subscale. NEO-FFI-C=NEO-FFI, Conscientiousness Subscale. NCS-SF=Need
    For Cognition Scale—Short Form. EBI=Epistemic Beliefs Inventory. HCTAES=Halpern
    Critical Thinking Assessment Using Everyday Situations.
    *pb.05. **pb .01.

    possible joint effects with cognitive or dispositional factors. Together
    cognitive ability and epistemic beliefs contributed to 37% of the variance
    in critical thinking performance, of which beliefs contributed 11%
    unique variance.

    To sum up, results from Study 1 provided evidence for three major
    dimensions of epistemic beliefs, with omniscient authority and quick
    learning not emerging as distinct factors. This converged with findings
    from an earlier study by Nussbaum and Bendixen (2003). There was
    also support for all the stated hypotheses to various extents, with the
    relationships between epistemic beliefs and cognitive ability, thinking
    dispositions, and thinking performance being specified as follows:

    (i) The more sophisticated belief in knowledge being tentative
    (uncertain) was associated with better cognitive ability.

    (ii) The more sophisticated belief in ability being malleable (non-
    innate) was associated with a higher level of need for cognition,
    whereas the more sophisticated belief in knowledge being
    uncertain was associated with greater open-mindedness.

    Table 5
    Stepwise regression on critical thinking performance (Study 1, N=138).

    Predictor Critical thinking performance (standardized
    beta)

    Step 1 Step 2 Step 3 Step 4

    Demographics
    Gender −.07 −.01 −.03 −.14
    Age −.15 −.10 −.07 −.10
    SES −.03 .00 .01 .03

    Cognitive ability
    GPA .08 .11 .12
    VCI .46*** .45*** .41***

    Thinking dispositions
    Openness (NEO-FFI-O) −.05 −.16
    Conscientiousness (NEO-FFI-C) −.09 −.12
    Need for cognition (NCS-SF) .14 .07

    Epistemic beliefs (EBI)
    Innate ability −.19*
    Certain knowledge −.32***
    Simple knowledge −.14

    Multiple R2 .03 .24 .26 .37
    Increase in R2 .21 .02 .11

    Note. VCI=Verbal Comprehension Index of WAIS-III. NEO-FFI-O=NEO-FFI, Openness to
    Experience Subscale. NEO-FFI-C=NEO-FFI, Conscientiousness Subscale. NCS-SF=Need
    For Cognition Scale—Short Form. EBI=Epistemic Beliefs Inventory.
    *pb .05, ***pb .001.

    72 N.-M. Chan et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 21 (2011) 67–77

    (iii) The more sophisticated belief in knowledge being uncertain,
    and to a lesser extent also the belief in ability being malleable,
    was associated with better critical thinking performance.

    (iv) There were unique effects of epistemic beliefs on thinking
    performance, other than the possible effects exerted through
    their association with cognitive and dispositional factors.

    3. Study 2

    Results from Study 1 provided evidence that personal epistemology
    is related to cognitive ability and thinking disposition, and it produces
    unique effects on thinking performance. Study 2 aimed at further
    examining how, or the mechanisms through which epistemic beliefs
    exert influence on critical thinking performance, focusing on their
    effects on the argumentation process. More specifically, we investigated
    students’ reasoning process in face of a controversial issue where
    reasonable arguments and counterarguments were both available. This
    kind of tasks is typical of those calling for critical thinking. A more fine-
    grained examination of where individuals with naïve epistemic beliefs
    differ from their counterparts holding more sophisticated beliefs in this
    thinking process would throw further light on the exact nature of the
    epistemic belief-critical thinking relationship.

    As mentioned in the literature review, findings from previous studies
    have suggested a close-minded thinking style to be an important link
    between naïve epistemic beliefs and poor argumentation, epitomized in
    the tendency to draw simplistic or absolute conclusions based on
    inadequate considerations of opposing views or mixed evidence as well
    as the inclination to treat information that challenges preexisting beliefs
    in a biased manner. Results of our Study 1 also suggested that beliefs in
    ability and knowledge being fixed were associated with a reluctance to
    engage in thinking and be open-minded, at the same time predicting
    poorer performance on critical thinking tasks.

    When facing a controversial issue, which usually involves
    reasonable arguments supporting both sides of the debate, the
    drawing of sound conclusions would depend on an adequate
    consideration of arguments on both sides, i.e., two-sided thinking. A
    close-minded or absolutist thinking style will likely hamper this
    process by fixating only on one viewpoint and focusing on evidence
    that support their adopted position while ignoring counterarguments
    that do not. This explains why individuals who hold naïve epistemic
    beliefs tend to perform poorly in critical thinking, as critical thinkers
    should be able to effectively consider alternative viewpoints (Halpern,
    1998; Lewis & Smith, 1993). Based on this rationale the following
    hypotheses regarding the relationship between epistemic beliefs and
    argumentation performance were proposed and tested:

    (i) Participants with naïve epistemic beliefs are weaker in two-sided
    thinking, i.e., they are more likely to focus on the arguments
    supporting their position while overlooking counterarguments
    refuting their position.

    (ii) Participants with naïve epistemic beliefs exhibit a greater
    tendency to devaluate counterarguments, i.e., naïve beliefs will
    bias the participants’ thinking so that they will fail to objectively
    evaluate reasonable counterarguments, thus finding them less
    convincing than they are.

    (iii) Participants with naïve epistemic beliefs will be less efficient in
    tackling counterarguments, i.e., the lower tendency to consider
    and understand counterarguments would weaken one’s ability
    to consider arguments in a reasonable manner.

    3.1. Method

    3.1.1. Participants
    Participants were 111 Chinese undergraduates (35 males and

    76 females) in Hong Kong. Recruitment was done through making an
    announcement in Introductory Psychology courses. The age range was

    from 18 to 23 (M=19.85, SD=1.13) and their average GPA was
    2.81 (SD=.50). Participants consisted of students in their first
    (77.5%), second (16.2%) and final (6.3%) years of study with various
    major subjects. Participation was on a voluntary and anonymous
    basis.

    3.1.2. Thinking task
    A controversial issue was developed as the stimulus for the

    assessment of participants’ argumentation performance. In order to
    choose an issue that was appropriate for the study, a number of
    controversial issues were subject to several rounds of discussions and
    pilot-tested for their appropriateness. There were two criteria for
    judging appropriateness. Firstly, the number of people voting for and
    against the issue should be comparable. Secondly, there should be
    equally convincing arguments for and against the issue. Based on the
    results of the pilot study, an issue about whether long-term drug
    patent is acceptable was selected. Drug patents allow pharmaceutical
    companies to be the sole producer and freely set the price for a newly
    developed drug for 20 years. This arrangement safeguards the profit
    of the company and encourages the development of new drugs. Yet it
    also results in high drug prices which are not affordable by many who
    need the medicine. Therefore there has been a debate on whether this
    patent arrangement should be kept.

    In the pilot study, a group of graduate students (N=15) read a
    summary of the background information and the opposing views in
    the debate. Then they read two passages arguing for or against the
    patent arrangement. Participants were asked to rate the reasonable-
    ness of both passages on a 10-point Likert scale ranging from −5
    (“Totally unreasonable”) to 5 (“Totally reasonable”).Then they
    indicated their position for the issue, i.e., whether the drug patent
    arrangement should be kept. It was found that the two argumentative
    passages were both rated as reasonable by all the participants. Both
    the arguments for (M=3.47, SD=.92) and against (M=3.33,
    SD=1.11) the arrangement were rated as reasonable, with non-
    significant difference between the mean ratings for reasonableness of
    the two argumentative passages; t(14)=.358, n.s. For the final
    position, there were 53.33% participants indicating support for and
    46.67% indicating opposition of the specified patent arrangement.
    Thus the controversial nature of this issue was ensured, with the two
    passages arguing for or against it being comparable in reasonableness.

    3.1.3. Measures
    The adapted Epistemic Beliefs Inventory (EBI) used in Study 1 was

    employed in the present study to measure participants’ epistemic
    beliefs. It consists of three dimensions (innate ability, certain
    knowledge, simple knowledge) and a total of 12 items (see Study 1
    for details).

    Participants’ performance in argumentation was measured
    through essay tasks. They were assessed on (i) their ability to provide
    two-sided arguments in response to the controversial issue and
    (ii) their ability to tackle counterarguments presented to them.

    Participants first read the passage that summarized the debate on
    the issue. They were then asked to indicate their position regarding
    the patent arrangement (for or against) and write a short essay
    providing the reasoning behind their position (“Please explain your
    thinking and the relevant arguments behind your position”). They
    were also asked to rate their confidence in the correctness of their
    conclusion on a 5-point Likert scale (from 1 “Totally unconfident” to 5
    “Totally confident).

    After that, participants were given an argumentative passage
    which opposed their stated position and were asked to rate the
    convincingness of the counterarguments on a 5-point Likert scale (from
    1 “Totally unconvincing” to 5 “Very convincing”). Finally they were
    asked to re-consider their position and explain their thinking in
    writing (“After reading the article, would you change your stand on
    the issue? Explain why you would or would not change your stand”).

    73N.-M. Chan et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 21 (2011) 67–77

    A scoring rubric was developed to assess participants’ two-sided
    argument skills in their initial essays explaining the reasoning behind
    their position, with reference to the scheme developed by Ferretti,
    Macarthur, and Dowdy (2000) in their study of persuasive essays.
    Responses were rated from 1 to 6 according to their ability to
    recognize and address arguments for both sides of the debate and
    provide clear reasoning for their final conclusion (see Appendix A).
    Higher ratings indicated better two-sided argument skills. A random
    sample of one-third of the scripts was coded by another independent
    rater and the inter-rater reliability was found to be very good (Cohen’s
    kappa=.80). As for the tackling of counterarguments, another scoring
    rubric with a 3-point scale for rating responses was derived (see
    Appendix A). Higher scores indicated more competent tackling of
    counterarguments. Again an independent rater coded a random
    sample of one-third of the scripts and inter-rater reliability (Cohen’s
    kappa) was .71.

    3.1.4. Procedure
    All participants completed the procedures in one session, being

    administered the adapted EBI first and asked to provide background
    information on gender, age, and GPA. Then they were given the
    thinking task as described above.

    3.2. Results and discussion

    3.2.1. Preliminary analyses
    We started the analyses by again examining the factorial structure

    and classification of the 12 items for the adapted EBI, using
    confirmatory factor analysis. As indicated by the various goodness of
    fit indexes, the empirical data fitted the a priori 3-factor structure very
    well, χ2(51)=68.00, RMSEA=.055, NNFI=.90, CFI=.92, with all
    standardized factor loadings greater than .40. Internal consistency as
    measured by Cronbach’s alphas for Innate Ability, Certain Knowledge
    and Simple Knowledge was .68, .64, and .67 respectively. Again none
    of the inter-factor correlations were significant at the .05 level, with rs
    ranging from −.04 to .22. Thus further evidence was provided for the
    reliability of the inventory measuring three distinct dimensions of
    epistemic beliefs. Means and standard deviations of the variables are
    given in Table 2.

    3.2.2. Relationship between epistemic beliefs and argumentation
    Regression analyses were conducted to examine the extent to which

    epistemic beliefs predicted specific features in the argumentation
    process, namely (i) two-sided argument skills, (ii) confidence about

    Table 6
    Predicting argumentation performance with epistemic beliefs (Study 2, N=111).

    Criterion variable (standardized beta)

    Argumentation performance

    Predictor Two-sided argument skills Con

    Background
    Gender −.02 .0
    Age −.04 .1
    GPA .04 −.1

    Epistemic beliefs (EBI)
    Innate Ability −.03 .0
    Certain Knowledge −.35*** .0
    Simple Knowledge −.22* −.0

    Argumentation performance
    Two-sided argument skills .0
    Confidence

    Addressing counterargument

    Convincingness rating
    Tackling of counterargument

    Multiple R2 .17 .0

    Note. EBI=Epistemic Beliefs Inventory.
    *pb.05, ***pb .001.

    own conclusion, (iii) attitude towards reasonable counterarguments
    (convincingness rating), and (iv) tackling of counterarguments. Gender,
    age and GPA as an index of general ability were also included in the
    regression equations so as to control for their possible effects on various
    aspects of argumentation.

    Results (Table 6) showed that the certainty dimension of
    epistemological beliefs, and to a lesser extent also the simplicity
    dimension, significantly predicted weaker two-sided argumentation.
    Therefore Hypothesis 1 for the present study was supported. The
    belief in knowledge being certain also predicted a greater tendency to
    devaluate reasonable counterarguments, thus providing support for
    Hypothesis 2. However, these beliefs did not seem to be related
    to how well students tackled counterarguments, apparently not
    supporting Hypothesis 3. Instead, it was found that students who
    were more confident about their viewpoint were slightly more likely
    to tackle counterarguments with good reasoning.

    In order to further explore possible differences in the treatment of
    counterarguments between people holding naïve beliefs and those
    holding sophisticated beliefs, more detailed analyses were conducted
    with selected groups of participants. Specifically, participants were
    divided into three groups based on their scores on the Certain
    Knowledge Scale, and comparisons were made between the group
    with highest scores (top one-third constituting the naïve group,
    N=44) and the group with lowest scores (bottom one-third making
    up the sophisticated group, N=35). Group membership was based on
    scores on the Certain Knowledge dimension because this dimension
    was found to be most salient in influencing critical thinking in
    previous and the present studies. The distribution of the types of
    responses given by these two extreme groups in their treatment of
    counterarguments is given in Table 7.

    A chi-square test showed a significant uneven distribution of
    the types of responses given by these two groups; Pearson chi-square
    (2)=6.33, pb.05; Fisher’s Exact Test=6.32, pb .05. On closer
    examination, there was a greater percentage of people among the
    naïve group (43%) ignoring or failing to address counterarguments
    than among the sophisticated group (17%). In contrast, students
    holding more sophisticated beliefs were more likely to address
    counterarguments, whether adequately or inadequately (a total of
    83%), than their naïve counterparts (a total of 57%). Worthy to note is
    that the proportion of people being able to address counterarguments
    adequately was actually quite comparable between the naïve group
    and the sophisticated group (23% versus 29%, respectively). This
    pattern of results suggested that the naïve group and the sophisti-
    cated group differed more in attention to counterarguments rather

    Addressing counterargument

    fidence Convincingness rating

    Tackling counterargument

    2 −.14 −.08
    1 .04 .10
    1 .02 −.10

    1 .01 .13
    1 −.24* .06
    9 .11 .10

    9 .09 .13
    −.02 .19*

    .12

    4 .11 .15

    Table 7
    Rebuttals generated by the naïve and sophisticated groups (frequencies and percentages).

    Tackling of counterargument Belief in certain knowledge

    Naïve group Sophisticated group

    1. Failing to address counterargument 19 (43%) 6 (17%)
    2. Underdeveloped rebuttal 15 (34%) 19 (54%)
    3. Sensible rebuttal 10 (23%) 10 (29%)
    Subtotal 44 (100%) 35 (100%)

    74 N.-M. Chan et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 21 (2011) 67–77

    than in their ability to tackle them. In view of this, the regression
    analysis result of a non-significant relationship between epistemic
    beliefs and performance in the tackling of counterarguments could
    perhaps be attributed to the lack of differentiation between attention
    and ability factors in the measure of this performance. This analysis of
    the distribution of different treatments of counterarguments and the
    corresponding chi-square test results did provide some support for
    Hypothesis 3.

    4. Summary and concluding discussion

    The present studies aimed at examining the relationship between
    epistemic beliefs and critical thinking among Chinese students, and
    the results largely conformed to related theorizations in the Western
    context. In particular, the unique contribution of these beliefs on
    thinking was highlighted. Results from Study 1 showed that these
    beliefs had unique effects on critical thinking performance, over and
    above those produced by cognitive ability. Specifically, the belief in
    knowledge being fixed and absolute (certain knowledge), to a lesser
    extent also the belief that our ability to process knowledge is fixed
    (innate ability), significantly predicted poorer performance on
    everyday evaluative thinking. It was also found that this belief in
    the certainty of knowledge was associated with closed-mindedness
    and poorer cognitive ability, whereas the belief in fixed ability was
    related to a lower need for cognition. It appears that while poor
    cognitive skills contribute to poor thinking performance, the lack of
    recognition of the tentative nature of knowledge, together with the
    lack of a conviction that effort and practice would improve our ability
    to think, further hampers critical thinking performance.

    In Study 2, it was further demonstrated that when thinking about
    controversial issues, beliefs about knowledge being certain and simple
    would deter one from considering issues from multiple perspectives
    and make him/her dismiss counterarguments more readily. This
    tendency to engage in one-sided thinking would produce detrimental
    effects on argumentation performance. In conclusion, the belief in
    knowledge being certain has been highlighted in two studies as the
    most significant aspect of personal epistemology affecting everyday
    evaluative thinking and the more specific process of argumentation.

    These results throw light on the nature of epistemic effects on
    critical thinking. Naïve beliefs about knowledge predispose one to a
    fixated and single-perspective approach to issues, thus affecting
    the integration and generation of knowledge during thinking. In
    particular, the lack of multiple perspectives hampers the ability to
    generate and judge alternatives in decision-making and problem-
    solving, which characterizes good critical thinking (Ennis, 1991;
    Halpern, 1998). Moreover, the tendency to devalue reasonable
    counterarguments reflects a poor ability to reason independently of
    one’s prior belief bias, which has also been emphasized to be an
    important critical thinking skill (Sá et al., 1999; Stanovich & West,
    1997).

    The present results also provide support for the postulation of
    reciprocal relationships among epistemic beliefs, cognitive ability, and
    thinking dispositions. While sophistication in epistemology was found
    to be related to cognitive ability and desirable thinking dispositions in
    Study 1, the fine-grained analysis of argumentation processes in Study 2

    pointed further to the nature of these relationships. People holding
    absolutist beliefs (knowledge being certain and simple) tend to reach a
    position on controversial issues quickly rather than spending effort on
    the consideration and objective evaluation of opposing arguments. This
    tendency would reinforce a close-minded thinking habit. Moreover,
    although individuals with naïve beliefs may have the ability to pro-
    vide sensible rebuttals for counterarguments, the inclination of not
    addressing counterargument would prevent them from exercising and
    refining this ability. Handling problems in these ways is not adaptive as
    it makes the revision of one’s reasoning difficult. As a result of these
    cyclical processes, epistemic beliefs, thinking ability and thinking
    dispositions would develop in synchrony.

    Overall, the present findings highlight an important link between
    naïve epistemic beliefs and poor critical thinking, namely the
    tendency to draw quick conclusions without adequate consideration
    of alternative views. This observation is consistent with findings about
    the cognitive characteristics of people holding naïve epistemic beliefs
    in previous research, in which other thinking performances were
    examined with different methodologies (Kardash & Howell, 2000;
    Kardash & Scholes, 1996; Qian & Alvermann, 1995; Schommer, 1990;
    Schommer-Aikins & Hutter, 2002; Sinatra et al., 2003). It also
    corroborates other findings about the relationship between cognitive
    dispositions and personal epistemology: an open-minded approach is
    positively correlated with the belief that decision-making takes time
    and effort (Kardash & Sinatra, 2003), and a mature and systematic
    thinking style is associated with the belief in knowledge being
    tentative and evolving (Valanides & Angeli, 2008).

    While the present findings indicate that the postulated relation-
    ships between epistemic beliefs and critical thinking largely apply in
    both Western and Chinese contexts, the fact that beliefs in knowledge
    being certain are of particular importance warrants attention. In the
    Chinese–Confucian culture, high values are placed on respect for
    authority, tradition and social order, predisposing members of the
    culture to cognitive conservatism (Ho, 1994). For an individual to put
    aside preconceptions and consider issues from alternative perspec-
    tives, a strong belief in the tentative nature of knowledge is needed. In
    other words, in a society where following established views is
    encouraged and arguments are discouraged, the importance of a belief
    in the tentative nature of knowledge as a precursor to good critical
    thinking is highlighted, as the present results show.

    4.1. Implications for practice

    Programs designed for the enhancement of critical thinking
    typically focus on the teaching of related thinking skills. The present
    results suggest that fostering sophistication in students’ epistemic
    beliefs is also conducive to critical thinking development. Specifically,
    students should be encouraged to recognize the tentative and
    complex nature of knowledge as well as the fact that practice and
    effort could help improve our ability to think. This has special
    significance for the nurturing of critical thinking in Chinese and other
    societies sharing the Confucian cultural heritage. As mentioned above,
    the tendency of cognitive conservatism in this culture may pose
    significant hindrance to people’s engagement in analytical thinking.
    Without the simultaneous cultivation of sophisticated epistemic
    beliefs, the effects of critical thinking skills training may be limited.

    Western research has shown that epistemological development
    could be fostered by the experience of epistemic doubt, where one
    questions the existence of absolute knowledge (Bendixen, 2002). An
    effective way to induce epistemic doubt is to have students reflect and
    judge on ill-structured problems so that they become more aware of
    alternative modes of thought (King & Kitchener, 2002; Kuhn, 2001).
    For example, Kuhn, Shaw, and Felton (1997) found that students’
    metacognitive awareness of the co-existence of multiple viewpoints
    could be enhanced through dyadic discussions of controversial issues.

    75N.-M. Chan et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 21 (2011) 67–77

    Having students debate over controversial issues would also
    allow opportunities for practicing the generation and evaluation of
    counterarguments. As our results show, the tendency to devalue
    counterarguments that may be reasonable is a major hindrance for
    students holding naïve beliefs to think critically. At the end of the
    debate, students should draw their own conclusion based on the
    supporting arguments as well as their explanations for the counter-
    arguments being inferior to the supporting arguments. Such activities
    would not only foster the development of multiple perspectives but
    also promote the essential critical thinking skill of assessing subjective
    viewpoints based on objective criteria of good reasoning.

    An additional implication for practice is that Chinese educators,
    who may succumb more to the knowledge-transmission conception
    of teaching and learning (Chai, Khine, & Teo, 2006; Chan & Elliot,
    2004b), should be more aware of instructional practices that foster
    naïve beliefs in the certainty of knowledge. For example, questioning
    that focuses too much on soliciting factual information would
    reinforce the belief that knowledge is absolute (Kurfiss, 1988). In
    the same vein, “textbooks that present subject matter as non-
    problematic reinforce dualistic thinking” (Kurfiss, 1988, p. 64). The
    common use of assessment methods with restrictive answers might
    also cue students to search for model answers when they face
    problems and regard those answers as absolute. For example,
    multiple-choice questions implicitly reinforce the concept that only
    one correct answer exists. Where appropriate, using open-ended
    methods to assess students’ learning progress might be more desirable
    for the promotion of epistemological development. Generally speaking,
    constructivist learning approaches characterized by the active con-
    struction of knowledge through critical analysis would have more
    positive impacts on students’ epistemological and cognitive develop-
    ment (Marra, Palmer, & Litzinger, 2000; Pascarella & Terenzini, 1991;
    Stephenson & Hunt, 1977; Valanides & Angeli, 2005).

    4.2. Limitations and further research

    The present studies provide clear evidence that epistemological
    beliefs are related to critical thinking performance, and the results
    largely converge with findings from related studies in Western
    contexts. However, more research is needed to examine how
    epistemic beliefs interact with cognitive ability and thinking disposi-
    tions to produce effects on critical thinking. Clarifying these relation-
    ships would help build a more precise and comprehensive theory of
    critical thinking, at the same time generating insights into the
    effective enhancement of critical thinking from multiple perspectives.
    In particular, conducting similar research in different cultural contexts
    would enable comparisons to be made and advance our understand-
    ing of cultural factors in thinking.

    In view of the limitations of quantitative studies using Likert scale
    measures, which assume that the variables could be captured on a
    continuous scale, it would be desirable to also conduct qualitative
    research (e.g., in-depth interviews) in an attempt to capture more
    clearly the complexity of changes in thinking and beliefs under
    different conditions. Discourse analysis using natural data on how
    students treat knowledge during thinking would also be fruitful.
    Employing mixed-method designs in research would enable broader
    perspectives in investigation and cross-validation of findings.

    As the ultimate value of understanding thinking-related processes
    is to inform about how thinking performance may be enhanced, more
    direct investigations on effective epistemological intervention would
    be desirable. Although implications for practice could be drawn based
    on available evidence such as those generated in the present studies,
    the conclusions drawn are largely only inferential in nature based on
    correlational data. Direct empirical evidence generated from exper-
    imental or longitudinal studies would help derive more definite
    assertions about effective intervention. For example, tracing how
    students’ thinking and beliefs develop during intervention or over

    time would provide valuable information. Issues that are worth
    exploring include the direction of causality of effects, the malleability
    of epistemic beliefs, and other possible intervening factors.

    To conclude, the present studies have provided evidence for the
    significant and unique role of personal epistemology in critical
    thinking among Chinese students. More specifically, naïve beliefs
    about knowledge limit one’s ability to consider alternatives from
    multiple perspectives and to objectively evaluate arguments that are
    not consistent with prior conceptions. These beliefs likely develop in
    synchrony with cognitive ability and desirable thinking dispositions,
    jointly producing effects on critical thinking. Future research should
    attempt to clarify the relative roles and relationships of these
    thinking-related factors and directly investigate intervention issues
    so as to specify more clearly the kind of epistemological intervention
    that would be effective for the enhancement of critical thinking.

    Acknowledgement

    Thepreparation of this manuscript was partially supported bya grant
    from the Research Grants Council of Hong Kong (Project no. 443809).

    Appendix A

    Scoring rubrics for argumentation performance in Study 2.

    Score

    Description
    Two-sided argument skills
    1

    No valid argument

    The arguments used to support the position are either unrelated
    to the topic or invalid.

    2
    Partially developed one-sided argument

    Although some valid arguments are stated to support the position,
    the exposition also contains other supporting arguments which are
    either unrelated to the topic or invalid.

    3
    Well-developed one-sided argument

    The exposition states supporting arguments which are valid and
    elaborated clearly.

    4
    Two-sided argument: only recognizing counterargument

    The exposition states supporting arguments which are elaborated
    clearly. In addition, it also mentions counterarguments but fails to give
    any refutations against them.

    5
    Two-sided argument: minimally addressing counterargument

    The exposition states supporting arguments which are elaborated
    clearly. Counterarguments are stated clearly but not addressed
    adequately with sensible rebuttal or alternative solutions.

    6
    Two-sided argument: well-developed argument addressing counterargument

    The exposition states supporting arguments which are elaborated
    clearly. Moreover, counterarguments are addressed either with sensible
    rebuttal or alternative solutions.

    Tackling counterargument
    1

    Failing to address counterargument

    Participant largely only restates own position and argument, without
    addressing the counterargument.

    2
    Underdeveloped rebuttal

    Participant attempts to address the counterargument but the reasoning
    or rebuttal is not well developed or is of limited plausibility.

    3
    Sensible rebuttal

    Participant provides a sensible evaluation of the counterargument and
    provides a reasonable conclusion.

    References

    American Philosophical Association. (1990). Critical thinking: A statement of expert
    consensus for purposes of educational assessment and instruction, (“The Delphi
    Report”). ERIC Document Reproduction No. ED 315 423.

    Bendixen, L. D. (2002). A process model of epistemic belief change. In B. K. Hofer, & P. R.
    Pintrich (Eds.), Personal epistemological: The psychology of beliefs about knowledge
    and knowing (pp. 191−208). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Bendixen, L. D., Schraw, G., & Dunkle, M. E. (1998). Epistemic beliefs and moral
    reasoning. The Journal of Psychology, 13, 187−200.

    Brabeck, M. (1983). Critical thinking skills and reflective judgment development:
    Redefining the aims of higher education. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology,
    4, 23−34.

    76 N.-M. Chan et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 21 (2011) 67–77

    Brown, A. L. (1997). Transforming schools into communities of thinking and learning
    about serious matters. The American Psychologist, 52(4), 399−413.

    Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., & Kao, C. F. (1984). The efficient assessment of need for
    cognition. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48(3), 306−307.

    Chai, C. S., Khine, M. S., & Teo, T. (2006). Epistemological beliefs on teaching and
    learning: A survey among pre-service teachers in Singapore. Educational Media
    International, 43, 285−298.

    Chan, K. W., & Elliot, R. G. (2004a). Epistemological beliefs across cultures: Critique and
    analysis of beliefs structure studies. Educational Psychology, 24, 123−142.

    Chan, K. W., & Elliot, R. G. (2004b). Relational analysis of personal epistemology and
    conceptions about teaching and learning. Teaching and Teacher Education, 20,
    817−831.

    Clifford, J. S., Boufal, M. M., & Kurtz, J. E. (2004). Personality traits and critical thinking
    skills in college students. Assessment, 11(2), 169−176.

    Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory and NEO Five Factor
    Inventory: Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

    Duell, O. K., & Schommer-Aikins, M. (2001). Measures of people’s beliefs about
    knowledge and learning. Educational Psychology Review, 13(4), 419−449.

    Ennis, R. H. (1991). Critical thinking: A streamlined conception. Teaching Philosophy, 14(1),
    5−25.

    Facione, P. A., Facione, N. C., & Giancarlo, C. A. (2000). The disposition toward critical
    thinking: Its character, measurement, and relationship to critical thinking skill.
    Informal Logic, 20(1), 61−84.

    Ferretti, R. P., Macarthur, C., & Dowdy, N. (2000). The effects of an elaborated goal on
    persuasive writing of students with learning disabilities and their normally
    achieving peers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(4), 694−702.

    Fischer, S. C., & Spiker, V. A. (2000). A framework for critical thinking research and training.
    Report prepared for the US Army Research Institute. Santa Barbara, California: Anacapa
    Sciences Inc.

    Gallagher, S. A. (1998). The road to critical thinking: The Perry scheme and meaningful
    differentiation. NASSP Bulletin, 82(595), 12−20.

    Halpern, D. F. (1998). Teaching critical thinking for transfer across domains:
    Dispositions, skills, structure training, and metacognitive monitoring. The American
    Psychologist, 53(4), 449−455.

    Halpern, D. F. (1999). Teaching for critical thinking: Helping college students develop
    the skills and dispositions of a critical thinker. New Directions for Teaching and
    Learning, 80, 69−74.

    Halpern, D. F. (2003). Thought and knowledge: An introduction to critical thinking (4th ed.).
    Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Halpern, D. F. (2007). Halpern Critical Thinking Assessment: Background and scoring
    standards. Unpublished manuscript. Claremont, CA: Claremont McKenna College.

    Hau, K. T., Halpern, D. F., Marin-Burkhard, L., Ho, I. T., Ku, K. Y. L., Chan, N. M., & Lun, V. M. C.
    (2006). Assessment of Chinese and U.S. students’ critical thinking: Open-ended versus
    forced-choice items. Unpublished manuscript. Hong Kong, China: The Chinese
    University of Hong Kong.

    Ho, D. Y. F. (1994). Filial piety, authoritarian moralism, and cognitive conservatism in
    Chinese societies. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 120,
    347−365.

    Hofer, B. K. (2001). Personal epistemology research: Implications for learning and
    teaching. Educational Psychology Review, 13(4), 353−383.

    Hofer, B. K. (2002). Personal epistemology as a psychological and educational construct:
    An introduction. In B. K. Hofer, & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Personal epistemological: The
    psychology of beliefs about knowledge and knowing (pp. 3−14). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Hofer, B. K. (2008). Personal epistemology and culture. In M. S. (Ed.), Knowing,
    knowledge, and beliefs: Epistemological studies across diverse cultures (pp. 3−22).
    Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Springer.

    Hofer, B. K., & Printrich, P. R. (1997). The development of epistemological theories:
    Beliefs about knowledge and knowing and their relation to learning. Review of
    Educational Research, 67(1), 88−140.

    Hui, N. H. H. (2003). Nomological network of need for cognition: A study with Chinese
    college students. Unpublished manuscript, Department of Psychology, Chinese
    University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong.

    Jehng, J. J., Johnson, S. D., & Anderson, R. C. (1993). Schooling and students’ epistemological
    beliefs about learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 18, 23−35.

    Jones, P. C., Merritt, J. Q., & Palmer, C. (1999). Critical thinking and interdisciplinarity in
    environmental higher education: The case for epistemological and values
    awareness. Journal of Geography in Higher Education, 23(3), 349−357.

    Kao, C. (1994). The concept and measurement of need for cognition. Chinese Journal of
    Psychology, 36, 1−20.

    Kardash, C. M., & Howell, K. L. (2000). Effects of epistemological beliefs and topic-
    specific beliefs on undergraduates’ cognitive and strategic processing of dual-
    positional text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(3), 524−535.

    Kardash, C. M., & Scholes, R. J. (1996). Effects of preexisting beliefs, epistemological
    beliefs, and need for cognition on interpretation of controversial issues. Journal of
    Educational Psychology, 88(2), 260−271.

    Kardash, C. M., & Sinatra, G. (2003). Epistemological beliefs and dispositions: Are we
    measuring the same construct? Poster paper presented at the Annual General
    Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Chicago, IL.

    Khine, M. S. (Ed.). (2008). Knowing, knowledge, and beliefs: Epistemological studies across
    diverse cultures. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Springer.

    King, P. M., & Kitchener, K. S. (2002). The reflective judgment model: Twenty years of
    research on epistemic cognition. In B. K. Hofer, & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Personal
    epistemological: The psychology of beliefs about knowledge and knowing (pp. 37−61).
    Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Ku, K. Y. L., & Ho, I. T. (2010). Dispositional factors predicting Chinese students’ critical
    thinking performance. Personality and Individual Differences, 48(1), 54−58.

    Kuang, Y., Shi, J. Q., Cai, Y. Q., & Wang, L. (2005). The Chinese version of need for
    cognition scale. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 19(1), 57−60.

    Kuhn, D. (1999). Metacognitive development. In L. Balter, & C. S. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.),
    Child psychology: A handbook of contemporary issues (pp. 259−286). Philadelphia,
    PA: Psychology Press.

    Kuhn, D. (2001). How do people know? Psychological Science, 12(1), 1−8.
    Kuhn, D., Shaw, V., & Felton, M. (1997). Effects of dyadic interaction on argumentative

    reasoning. Cognition and Instruction, 15(3), 287−315.
    Kurfiss, J. G. (1988). Critical thinking: Theory, research, practice, and possibilities.

    Washington, DC: Association for the Study of Higher Education.
    Lewis, A., & Smith, D. (1993). Defining higher order thinking. Theory into Practice, 32(3),

    131−137.
    Lin, C. H. (2001). Epistemological development and academic performance among

    elementary students. Journal of National Taichung Teachers College, 15, 191−206.
    Marra, R. M., Palmer, B., & Litzinger, T. (2000). The effects of a first-year engineering

    design course on student intellectual development as measured by the Perry
    scheme. Journal of Engineering Education, 89(1), 39−45.

    Mines, R. A., King, P. M., Hood, A. B., & Wood, P. K. (1990). Stages of intellectual
    development and associated critical thinking skills in college students. Journal of
    College Student Development, 31(6), 538−547.

    Muis, K. R. (2007). The role of epistemic beliefs in self-regulated learning. Educational
    Psychologist, 42, 173−190.

    Nussbaum, E. M., & Bendixen, L. D. (2003). Approaching and avoiding arguments: The
    role of epistemological beliefs, need for cognition, and extraverted personality
    traits. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28(4), 573−595.

    Pascarella, E. T., & Terenzini, P. T. (1991). How college affects students. San Francisco, CA:
    Jossey-Bass.

    Paul, R. W. (1984). Critical thinking: Fundamental to education for a free society.
    Educational Leadership, 42(1), 4−14.

    Perkins, D. N., Jay, E., & Tishman, S. (1993). Beyond abilities: A dispositional theory of
    thinking. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 39(1), 1−21.

    Qian, G., & Alvermann, D. (1995). Role of epistemological beliefs and learned
    helplessness in secondary school students’ learning science concepts from text.
    Journal of Educational Psychology, 87(2), 282−292.

    Qian, G., & Pan, J. (2002). A comparison of epistemological beliefs and learning from
    science text between American and Chinese high school students. In B. K. Hofer, &
    P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Personal epistemology: The psychology of beliefs about knowledge
    and knowing (pp. 365−385). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Sá, W. C., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (1999). The domain specificity and generality of
    belief bias: Searching for a generalizable critical thinking skill. Journal of Educational
    Psychology, 91(3), 497−510.

    Schommer, M. (1990). Effects of beliefs about the nature of knowledge on
    comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(3), 498−504.

    Schommer, M. (1994). An emerging conceptualization of epistemological beliefs and
    their role in learning. In R. Garner, & P. A. Alexander (Eds.), Beliefs about text and
    instruction with text (pp. 25−40). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

    Schommer-Aikins, M. (2002). An evolving theoretical framework for an epistemological
    belief system. In B. K. Hofer, & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Personal epistemology: The
    psychology of beliefs about knowledge and knowing (pp. 103−118). Mahwah, NJ:
    Erlbaum.

    Schommer-Aikins, M., & Hutter, R. (2002). Epistemological beliefs and thinking about
    everyday controversial issues. The Journal of Psychology, 136(1), 5−20.

    Schraw, G. (2001). Current themes and future directions in epistemological research: A
    commentary. Educational Psychology Review, 13(4), 451−464.

    Schraw, G., Bendixen, L. D., & Dunkle, M. E. (2002). Development and validation of the
    Epistemic Belief Inventory (EBI). In B. K. Hofer, & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Personal
    epistemology: The psychology of beliefs about knowledge and knowing (pp. 121−141).
    Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Schraw, G., Dunkle, M. E., & Bendixen, L. D. (1995). Cognitive processes in well-defined
    and ill-defined problem solving. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 9(6), 523−538.

    Schraw, G., & Sinatra, G. M. (2004). Epistemological development and its impact on
    cognition in academic domains. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 29(2),
    95−102.

    Sinatra, G. M., Southerland, S. A., McConaughy, F., & Demastes, J. W. (2003). Intentions
    and beliefs in students’ understanding and acceptance of biological evolution.
    Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 40(5), 510−528.

    Spector, P. E., Schneider, J. R., Vance, C. A., & Hezlett, S. A. (2000). The relation of
    cognitive ability and personality traits to assessment center performance. Journal of
    Applied Social Psychology, 30, 1474−1491.

    Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1997). Reasoning independently of prior belief and
    individual differences in actively open-minded thinking. Journal of Educational
    Psychology, 89(2), 342−357.

    Stephenson, B., & Hunt, C. (1977). Intellectual and ethical development: A dualistic
    curriculum intervention for college students. Counseling Psychologist, 6, 39−42.

    Toplak, M. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (2002). The domain specificity and generality of
    disjunctive reasoning: Searching for a generalizable critical thinking skill. Journal of
    Educational Psychology, 94, 197−209.

    Valanides, N., & Angeli, C. (2005). Effects of instruction on changes in epistemological
    beliefs. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 30(2), 314−330.

    Valanides, N., & Angeli, C. (2008). An exploratory study about the role of epistemological
    beliefs and dispositions on learners’ thinking about an ill-defined issue in solo and
    duo problem solving contexts. In M. S. Khine (Ed.), Knowing, knowledge, and beliefs:
    Epistemological studies across diverse cultures (pp. 197−218). Dordrecht, the
    Netherlands: Springer.

    Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition (Chinese Version) (2002). San Antonio,
    TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    77N.-M. Chan et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 21 (2011) 67–77

    Weinstock, M., & Cronin, M. A. (2003). The everyday production of knowledge:
    Individual differences in epistemological understanding and juror-reasoning skill.
    Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17(2), 161−181.

    West, R. F., Toplak, M. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (2008). Heuristics and biases as measures of
    critical thinking: Associations with cognitive ability and thinking dispositions.
    Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 930−941.

    Wood, P., & Kardash, C. (2002). Critical elements in the design and analysis of studies of
    epistemology. In B. K. Hofer, & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Personal epistemological: The
    psychology of beliefs about knowledge and knowing (pp. 231−260). Mahwah, NJ:
    Erlbaum.

    Zhang, L. F. (2002). Measuring thinking styles in addition to measuring personality
    traits? Personality and Individual Differences, 33(3), 445−458.

      Epistemic beliefs and critical thinking of Chinese students
      Introduction
      Epistemic beliefs
      Epistemic beliefs and critical thinking
      Effects of epistemic beliefs on argumentation
      Epistemic beliefs and other critical thinking factors
      The present research
      Study 1
      Method
      Participants
      Measures
      Procedure
      Results and discussion
      Preliminary analyses
      Relations between epistemic beliefs and other critical �thinking factors
      Epistemic beliefs as predictors of critical thinking performance

      Study 2
      Method
      Participants
      Thinking task
      Measures
      Procedure
      Results and discussion
      Preliminary analyses
      Relationship between epistemic beliefs and argumentation

      Summary and concluding discussion
      Implications for practice
      Limitations and further research
      Acknowledgement
      Appendix A
      References

    Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

    International Journal of Educational Research

    journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ijedures

  • Understanding and teaching critical thinking—A new approach
  • Kristoffer Larsson
    University of Gothenburg, Sweden

    A R T I C L E I N F O

    Keywords:
    critical thinking
    learning
    teaching
    pedagogy
    phenomenography

    A B S T R A C T

    Developing students’ critical thinking is a major educational goal in societies around the world.
    In spite of this, the research community has had serious problems handling this highly prized
    goal. In reference to these problems, several issues have been discussed, one being the theory
    issue, where the theoretical development has been pointed out as insufficient, especially theory
    concerning the learning experience connected with the development of critical thinking. This
    article introduces the use of the phenomenographic theoretical approach in the field of critical
    thinking. Taking an empirical study as an illustrative example, the article shows how phenom-
    enographic theory, in a promising way, could be used for understanding concrete expressions of
    critical thinking and designing teaching to develop students’ critical thinking.

    1. Introduction

    Developing students’ critical thinking skills is regarded as a highly important educational goal in many societies around the world,
    as it is seen as promoting such disparate qualities as democracy and personal development (Behar-Horenstein & Niu, 2011; Beyer,
    1995; Facione, 2006; Martin, 2005; Tsui, 1998). Despite the importance of critical thinking as an educational goal, the research
    community has had serious problems in tackling questions related to this topic. In 2004, Kuhn and Dean described critical thinking as
    one of the “major unsolved problems of pedagogy” (Kuhn & Dean, 2004:269) and over the years there have been several research
    reviews concluding that we know very little about critical thinking and how to promote it (Brunt, 2005; McMillan, 1987; Tsui, 1998).
    One of the more recent of these ended by appealing to the readers to “critically analyse the substance of empirical studies on teaching
    critical thinking” (Behar-Horenstein & Niu, 2011:

    38

    ).

    Against this background, several issues have been debated in the critical-thinking research community. Three of the more pro-
    minent ones have been the definition issue, the measuring issue and the theory issue.

    The definition issue concerns how to define critical thinking. It has been said that the phenomenon of critical thinking is an
    elusive and complex one, and that it is futile to attempt capture it in any definition (Brodin, 2007; Tsui, 2007). As a result of this,
    there has been an extensive debate on what critical thinking really is (Brodin, 2007; Johnson & Hamby, 2015; Mason, 2007; Moore,
    2013; Petress, 2004; Phillips & Bond, 2004; Tsui, 1998). One area of debate has concerned what to include and exclude in the
    definition. Is critical thinking just a set of skills or does it have dispositional aspects as well? Is creativity an important element or not
    and should emotions, intuition and societal change be brought into the definition (Brodin, 2007; Mason, 2007)? Another important
    area of debate has been whether critical thinking is subject specific or general (Mason, 2007; Tsui 1998, 2007). McPeck has argued
    that critical thinking is something subject specific, depending heavily on the subject that is to be critically thought about, and that
    every subject therefore has to find its own definition (McPeck 1990a, 1990b). Although admitting that the subject matter is involved,
    the strongest voices in the field, such as Ennis (1987, 1993) and Paul (1992), both long-term authorities in the field (Menssen, 1993),
    and Facione (1990), leader of the group that developed the APA/Delphi definition, have argued that there are indeed general

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2017.05.004
    Received 5 December 2016; Accepted 14 May 2017

    E-mail address: kristoffer.larsson@gu.se.

    International Journal of Educational Research 84 (2017) 32–

    42

    Available online 14 June 2017
    0883-0

    35

    5/ © 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

    MARK

    http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/08830355

    http://www.elsevier.com/locate/ijedures

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2017.05.004

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2017.05.004

    mailto:kristoffer.larsson@gu.se

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2017.05.004

    http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1016/j.ijer.2017.05.004&domain=pdf

    principles of critical thinking that have wide usability and go beyond specific subjects. They have consequently advocated generic
    definitions of critical thinking. Having failed to reach full closure on any of these questions, the debate still continues to this day.

    The measuring issue concerns how critical thinking skills are measured when studied empirically. Over the years, there has been a
    heavy reliance on standardised multiple-choice tests (Brunt, 2005; McMillan 1987; Norris 1986; Tsui 1998), such as the Cornell
    Critical Thinking Test and the Watson-Glaser Critical Thinking Appraisal (Behar-Horenstein & Niu, 2011; Tsui, 1998). There are
    indeed advantages to these kinds of tests. For example, they give clear-cut answers that do not allow for different interpretations
    when assessed, and so have high inter-rater reliability, and there is also the possibility of quite easily conducting large-scale studies,
    for example, measuring critical-thinking skills in big cohorts of compulsory school students, thus aiming at generalizability. The
    heavy reliance on these kinds of test, however, has also been criticised. One criticism has been that these tests can only handle what
    are called well-structured problems with logical solutions. That is not an unimportant part of critical thinking, but it leaves out what
    are called ill-structured problems, in short, problems where there is no definite answer and it is up to the individual to make
    judgements based on reasoning (King & Kitchener, 1994; Kuhn, 1991). Another criticism has been that these tests only measure
    product (right or wrong x-marks on multiple-choice questions) while neglecting process (how the application of critical-thinking
    skills led to the answers) (Norris, 1985; Tsui, 1998, 2002; Norris, Leighton, Phillips, 2004; Newmann, 1991; Larsson, In press).
    Against this background, suggestions have been made to increase the use of alternative measuring methods, such as open-ended essay
    tasks, when measuring critical thinking empirically (Brunt, 2005; Tsui, 1998). This kind of essay task has been said to make the
    students reveal their critical-thinking skills, i.e. the essay format forces the students to make their reasoning processes explicit. These
    kinds of tasks are also better suited to ill-structured problems, because they can be formulated to allow for uncertainty, postponed
    conclusions etc. (King & Kitchener, 1994, 2004; Kuhn, 1991). However, from a measuring point of view, one problem with such tasks
    is that they allow the possibility of different interpretations when assessed, making the inter-rater reliability somewhat troublesome.
    This, coupled with the fact that they are more suited to small-scale studies, because the empirical data for analysis soon becomes
    voluminous, makes for studies with a lower degree of generalizability.

    The theory issue was raised by McMillan, in 1987. In his research review, he observed a lack of theoretical development within
    the field, especially concerning the “theoretical description of the nature of the learning experience” in connection to the develop-
    ment of critical thinking (McMillan, 1987:14). Over the years, there have been some attempts to meet his criticism. Kuhn is one
    researcher who has connected her studies to theories from a cognitive development tradition (Kuhn & Dean, 2004; Kuhn, 1991,
    1999). In short, her view rests on an assumption that an individual’s skill in thinking critically rests on that individual’s mental
    models on a meta-level, the level where models are selected and their use is monitored (Kuhn & Dean, 2004). To become more
    qualified in critical thinking, a person has to change her/his mental models to more advanced ones. This could be, for example, a
    change in the mental model of causality, from a co-occurrence causality model to a multivariable model of causality. Kuhn affirms
    that this change is dependent both on maturity and on learning experiences. In designing learning experiences that promote this kind
    of change, it is important not to focus merely on the direct performance level, for example describing what a multivariable model of
    causality is and how to use it. Instead the learning experience should be directed towards the mental models on a meta-level. For
    example, with regard to the causality models, the learning experience should be designed to focus on why the multivariable model of
    causality is preferable and why the other model is less effective, so that the multivariable model becomes the preferred choice for the
    individual (Kuhn & Dean, 2004). Another pair of researchers who have connected their studies on critical thinking to a cognitive
    development tradition are King and Kitchener (King & Kitchener, 1994, 2004). Though close to Kuhn, King and Kitchener focus
    purely on the individual’s assumptions about knowledge and how knowledge is acquired, and link this to an individual’s ability to
    think critically. In their model, there are seven different hierarchically ordered views of knowledge, linked to seven hierarchically
    ordered skills in thinking critically, where only the two most advanced views of knowledge actually allow for critical thinking.
    According to King and Kitchener, a change in one’s view of knowledge (and consequently critical thinking) is due to both maturity
    and learning experiences. In order to create learning experiences that facilitate more advanced views of knowledge, King and
    Kitchener propose that the experience should be designed to challenge the individual’s current view of knowledge. If this is suc-
    cessfully done, the individual becomes aware of the inadequacy of his/her current view of knowledge and over time changes his/her
    view to a more advanced one. This change concurrently makes his/her critical thinking more complex (King & Kitchener, 1994).

    Though attempts, such as the ones mentioned above, have been made to give a theoretical description of learning experiences
    connected to the development of critical thinking, many studies (Sendag & Ferhan Odabasi, 2009; Tsui, 2002; Wheeler & Collins,
    2003; Chowning, Griswold, Kovarik, & Collins, 2012; Fung, 2014) still lack a clear-cut and thoroughly developed theory to describe
    changes in critical thinking due to different types of learning experiences. On the whole, McMillan’s observation from three decades
    ago still holds true.

    Using an empirical study as an illustrative example, this article focuses on the theory issue. The article introduces phenomeno-
    graphy (Marton & Booth, 1997; Marton, 1981) as a new approach in the field, with a theory that can be used both to understand
    manifestations of critical thinking and, building on such understandings, to describe and explain learning experiences that can
    enhance critical thinking among students. In this way, the article aims to contribute to theoretical development within the field and
    to suggest an alternative approach, rooted in the phenomenological and hermeneutical traditions, to those approaches that have been
    tried so far (Sandberg, 1996). Grounded in interpretative traditions, the phenomenographic approach uses qualitative methods to
    obtain data for its analysis. In the study presented in this article, open-ended essay tasks were used. As such, the article also indirectly
    addresses the measuring issue, showing how these kinds of essay tasks could be used and so gives one alternative to the predominant
    usage of standardised multiple-choice tests within the field.

    In the article, the phenomenographic approach and its conceptual framework will be developed alongside the empirical study and
    the implications of the results.

    K. Larsson International Journal of Educational Research 84 (2017) 32–42

    33

    2. The empirical study

    2.1. Aim

    The study I will take as my point of departure revolves around lower secondary-school students solving an essay task designed to
    elicit critical thinking about the philosopher Robert Nozick’s views of justice.

    Within the phenomenographic approach it is an axiom that there is only a limited number of distinctly different ways in which
    people are capable of understanding, seeing or experiencing any specific phenomenon they meet (Marton & Booth, 1997). Those
    different ways of understanding a phenomenon are logically related and as a rule they can also, in an educational setting, be
    hierarchically ordered in relation to a norm or an educational goal (Marton & Booth, 1997). If the phenomenon consists of a task, like
    the one on Nozick’s views on justice, that calls for critical thinking, then according to this assumption, there can only be a limited
    number of hierarchically ordered different ways of understanding the task among people at a collective level. Furthermore phe-
    nomenography makes the assumption that an ability to act in a certain way in relation to a phenomenon is delimited by a certain way
    of understanding that specific phenomenon (Marton & Booth, 1997; Marton, 1996; Marton, Runesson, Tsui, 2004). This means that
    each of the different ways of understanding a particular phenomenon delimits a specific way of acting in relation to it. If the
    phenomenon consists of a task that calls for critical thinking it could then be said that each of the different ways of understanding the
    task delimits a specific way of handling it, or more precisely, a specific type of critical thinking in relation to the task. In the light of
    the hierarchical order among the different ways of understanding such a task, it can also be said that a more powerful way of
    understanding delimits a more powerful type of critical thinking in relation to the task. Similarly, a less powerful way of under-
    standing the task delimits a less powerful type of critical thinking in relation to the task.

    With this as a background, the aim of the study was:

    • To reduce and describe the different hierarchically ordered ways of understanding the task and the types of critical thinking they
    delimit.

    • To use the findings from the first aim to discuss how to improve students critical thinking using phenomenographic learning and
    teaching theory.

    2.2. Method

    2.2.1. Definition of critical thinking
    As stated in the introduction, there has been a great deal of debate on how to conceptualise critical thinking. This study was

    designed to connect to Robert Ennis’s generic definition of critical thinking (Ennis, 1987, 1993). This decision was based on three
    factors. Firstly, the article argues for a use of phenomenography at a general level when conducting research and designing teaching
    on critical thinking, using the presented study, with its particular content, only as an example of how to use the approach. Such an
    intent makes the generic property of the Ennis definition suitable Secondly, the Ennis definition is one of the most recognised
    definitions of critical thinking in the field, and therefore connecting the study to this definition links the study to the overall body of
    work within the field (Masson, 2007; Menssen, 1993; Moore, 2013). Thirdly, most other well-established definitions within the field
    are generic in nature, for example, the Rickard Paul definition (1992) and the APA/Delphi definition (Facione, 1990), the latter
    having been formulated by 46 of the USA’s and Canada’s leading critical-thinking experts.

    Ennis defines critical thinking as “reasonable reflective thinking focused on deciding what to believe or do”. It is indeed a very
    dense definition and Ennis makes more than 150 different specifications on what this definition actually means (Ennis, 1987).
    However, Ennis also makes an abridgment consisting of only 10 specifications (Ennis, 1993). This study was designed to be in
    reasonable accordance with 4 of the specifications in the abridgment, namely a person’s ability to:

    1. Identify conclusions, reasons, and assumptions.
    2. Judge the quality of an argument, including the acceptability of its reasons, assumptions, and evidence.
    3. Develop and defend a position on an issue.
    4. Draw conclusions when warranted, but with caution (Ennis, 1993:180).

    2.2.2. The task
    The task began with the students reading a text introducing some of Robert Nozick’s thoughts on justice (see Appendix A). Based on

    this text the students were then asked to scrutinize Nozick’s standpoints. In this they were urged to reason about strengths and weak-
    nesses in his way of arguing for his standpoints and to explain why they saw a strength as a strength and a weakness as a weakness.

    2.2.3. Task design
    Several considerations were made when designing the task. For example, the text, as well as the wording of the actual task, were

    designed to be age-appropriate. Moreover, the task was created to be in reasonable accordance with the four specifications from the
    Ennis definition. The task was also formulated as an essay task, giving access to the student’s reasoning process and so making it
    possible to analyse from a phenomenographic point of view (Newmann, 1991; Norris et al., 2004; Norris, 1985; Tsui, 1998, 2002;
    Larsson, In press). Finally, on a continuum between well-structured and ill-structured problems, the task was designed to be close to

    K. Larsson International Journal of Educational Research 84 (2017) 32–42

    34

    the ill-structured end of the continuum (King & Kitchener, 1994; Kuhn, 1991).

    2.2.4. Participants
    The students participating in the study were aged 15–16 and were in their last year in the Swedish compulsory school. There was a

    total of 19 participants (labelled S1-S19), 11 girls and 8 boys: 6, 8 and 5 of them had respectively the lowest, medium and highest
    grades in civics; and they came from 3 different schools. The number of students participating was decided upon using empirical
    saturation, i.e. new students were added until there was no more relevant variation within in the targeted group. In order to provide a
    theoretical sample of a broader population, the composition of the students group was based on a desire to have a variation in sex,
    prior achievement and school attended (Ojo & Booth, 2009). Because the task relied heavily on reading and writing ability, students
    who had recently immigrated to Sweden and students with dyslexia were excluded from participating.

    2.2.5. Implementation
    The empirical study was implemented in such a way that each student worked alone on separate occasions, without any time

    limit, to solve the task. Each student wrote down their response to the task in a word file on a computer that was blocked for internet
    access. There were no other tools available to solve the task except for a dictionary and pencil and paper if the student wanted to
    make notes. While the student solved the task, I sat in an adjoining room and from time to time I looked in to check that everything
    was alright.

    2.2.6. Empirical analysis
    To derive the different ways in which the students understood the task about Robert Nozick’s views on justice a phenomeno-

    graphic analysis of the students’ written answers was made (Sandberg, 1994). The possibility of doing this rests on the theoretical
    assumption mentioned above, namely that the act − i.e. the answer or, so to say, the critical thinking in relation to the task − is
    delimited by a certain way of understanding the task (Marton & Booth, 1997; Marton et al., 2004; Marton, 1996). In other words, the
    different answers to a task reflect the limited number of ways of understanding the task. The analysis set out to discover these
    different ways of understanding the task, to group them together in a category system of logically related hierarchically ordered ways
    of understanding and to present them in what, in phenomenographic terms, is called an outcome space (Marton & Booth, 1997). At a
    global level, the key question in deriving the categories was: “For the student to arrive at this answer, how would he or she have
    understood the task?” (Marton & Säljö 1997 [1984]; Svensson 1997 [1984]). The analysis in itself involves an iterative process of
    reading the essay-answers repeatedly, interpreting and reinterpreting, looking at similarities and differences, grouping and re-
    grouping them in order to finally put what emerges as the same understanding of the task in the same group (Booth & Ingerman,
    2002; Marton & Booth, 1997; Sandberg, 1994). As this is an interpretive process, the results are not seen as incontrovertible, but
    should provide a fair and well-founded picture of the different ways of understanding the task represented in the empirical data
    (Booth & Ingerman, 2002).

    2.3. Empirical results

    From the data analysis, an outcome space was established with an internal structure that can be illustrated as in Fig. 1. In total,
    five different ways of understanding the task were found, ranging from what are here labelled A to E, each of which leads to a
    particular type of critical thinking, also labelled A to E, where E is more complex than D, D is more complex than C, and so on, to the
    least complex, A.

    To give a better understanding of the outcome space, I will now turn to a more detailed description of the five different ways of
    understanding the task, linking them to empirical extracts from the students’ answers, i.e. a certain type of critical thinking.

    To be able to do this, it is first necessary to make some further theoretical distinctions.
    From a phenomenographic point of view, the most important characteristics of a specific way of understanding a phenomenon

    relate to what is called the internal horizon (Marton & Booth, 1997). The internal horizon is constituted of the components or parts of
    the phenomenon that are being discerned and how these components are organized (Marton & Booth, 1997). Thus a certain way of
    understanding a phenomenon is constituted of a whole of discerned components and their internal organization. In view of the
    hierarchical structure of the category system, this ordinarily means that a more complex way of understanding the phenomenon
    consists of a whole of more relevant discerned components with a higher degree of internal organization, in relation to an educational
    norm, usually equivalent to the scientifically established norm of understanding, for the phenomenon in question. Similarly, a less
    complex way of understanding the phenomenon is seen as a whole with fewer relevant components discerned and with a less
    integrated organization in relation to the norm (Marton & Booth, 1997).

    In line with these theoretical claims, the analysis to derive the different ways of understanding the Nozick task targeted the
    internal horizon of a way of understanding and ordered its constituents hierarchically according to a norm. This means that each
    different way of understanding the task is described in terms of a whole of discerned components and their organization. This also
    means that a more complex way of understanding the task consists of a whole of more relevant discerned components with a higher
    degree of internal organization in relation to the norm, as compared to a less complex way of understanding the task. Each specific
    way of understanding the task also delimits a certain type of critical thinking, made explicit in the empirical extracts from which the
    specific understanding was educed. In the case of this study, the preferred norm was found among the 19 students participating and
    equates to the way of understanding the task that delimits the most elaborate critical thinking in relation to the task within this group
    of students. It should be noticed that using the participating students as a standard for establishing the norm could mean that there

    K. Larsson International Journal of Educational Research 84 (2017) 32–42
    35

    might be other more advanced ways of understanding the task (F, G and so on) to be found, for instance, if older age groups had been
    included in the study. As indicated above, this way of establishing the norm is not the most common one within phenomenography.
    However, it was used due to the fact that there is no pre-established, undisputed scientific norm regarding how the Nozick task is to
    be understood.

    What now follows is a description of each different way of understanding the task, starting with the least complex way of
    understanding the task, i.e. Way of understanding A. Each way of understanding is described in terms of its components, and for
    details on these, see Table 1.

    Way of understanding A
    In Way of understanding A, the task is understood as one of describing some of Nozick’s standpoints on justice. The central

    components are here limited to the standpoint component and the describing component. A typical student answer, i.e. the type of
    critical thinking from which this way of understanding was derived, looked like this:

    “Robert Nozick’s argument is that if we don’t let all individuals fight for their own welfare, then individuals will not make an effort
    and live on others instead. According to him, it’s not fair if you’re not allowed to keep what you worked for (for example, if the
    state takes your property).” (S4)1

    Way of understanding B
    In way of understanding B, the task is understood as one of scrutinizing one or more of Nozick’s standpoints by using one’s own

    values or standpoints.
    In comparison to Way of understanding A, this represents a significant shift in the quality of the understanding of the task from

    simply describing standpoints to in fact scrutinizing them. If it is linked to the definition of critical thinking used in the study, one
    could even say that this shift in understanding between A and B marks the borderline between a way of understanding the task that
    allows no actual critical thinking and a way of understanding the task that does allow for some basic kind of critical thinking and not
    just passive description. The central components in Way of understanding B are limited to the standpoint component, the scrutinizing

    Way of
    understanding E

    Way of
    understanding D

    Way of
    understanding C

    Way of
    understanding B

    Way of
    understanding A

    Critical thinking
    type E

    Critical thinking
    type C

    Critical thinking
    type B

    Critical thinking
    type A

    Critical thinking
    type D

    Fig. 1. Illustration of the internal structure of the outcome space concerning the task about Robert Nozick’s view on justice. To accentuate that it is the way of
    understanding the task that delimits the type of critical thinking in relation to the task, the different types of critical thinking are shown in dotted outlines.

    1 This empirical extract, and those presented below, have been translated from Swedish to English by the author. An effort has been made to stay as close to the
    original texts as possible. For distribution of individuals across categories, see Appendix B.

    K. Larsson International Journal of Educational Research 84 (2017) 32–42

    36

    component and the value component. A typical answer, i.e. the type of critical thinking from which this way of understanding was
    derived, looked like this:

    “The strength in Robert Nozick’s argumentation is the right to own things and the right to life. I think it’s a strength because it’s
    important to be able to have a life of your own and the right to own what you legally bought, etc. I think the weakness in his
    argumentation is that the state must never take taxes from the rich and give to the poor. I think it’s a weakness because the rich
    hardly need as much money as they have so why not help someone who does not have it so good.” (S8)

    Way of understanding C
    Way of understanding C shows the same kind of understanding as the previous one (B) but makes one further distinction within

    understanding the scrutinizing part of the task. This distinction is characterised by an understanding that opens up for examining one
    broader implication, other than the ones expressed in the text on Nozick (see Appendix A), of one of Nozick’s standpoints. In addition
    to discerning the same components as Way of understanding B, Way of understanding C then also discerns one additional component
    delimited as the broader implication component. A typical answer from which this way of understanding was derived manifested
    critical thinking connected to all four components of the understanding. The extract below is a part of such an answer. The underlined
    segment in the answer shows where the broader implication component was derived from.

    “Nozick’s strength is that if you own something then it’s yours and nobody can take it away from you, which of course makes it

    Table 1
    Summarized illustration of the different ways of understanding the task concerning Robert Nozick’s view on justice, complemented with derived educationally critical
    differences (ecds). The different ways of understanding are hierarchically ordered with regard to the internal horizon. “Way of understanding E” is the most complex
    way of experiencing the task. “Way of understanding A” is the least complex. The italic heading points to the most significant characteristic of the specific way of
    understanding in relation to the others.

    Way of Understanding E
    “To scrutinize by drawing attention to an inner contradiction between two of Nozick’s standpoints”
    The discernment of direct standpoints of Nozick’s view in line with the spirit of Nozick’s standpoints (standpoint component). The discernment of the main objective

    of the task as one of scrutinizing one/some of Nozick’s standpoints (scrutinizing component). The discernment of personal values/standpoints in relation to one/
    some of Nozick’s standpoints and offering opinions on it/them based on personal values/standpoints (value component). The discernment that a specific
    standpoint may have broader implications for society/groups/individuals (in addition to those directly treated in the text), if society were organized according
    to Nozick’s standpoints (broader implication component). Regarding such a specific broader implication, the discernment that this broader implication can be
    explained by argument and conclusion (argument/conclusion component). The discernment that (at least) two of Nozick’s standpoints may be set against one
    another and that there may therefore be an internal contradiction in Nozick’s reasoning (contradiction component).

    Educationally critical differences (ecds): the contradiction component

    Way of Understanding D
    “To scrutinize by conclusion and argument regarding a broader implication of some of Nozick’s standpoints”
    The discernment of direct standpoints of Nozick’s view in line with the spirit of Nozick’s standpoints (standpoint component). The discernment of the main objective

    of the task as one of scrutinizing one/some of Nozick’s standpoints (scrutinizing component). The discernment of personal values/standpoints in relation to one/
    some of Nozick’s standpoints and offering opinions on it/them based on personal values/standpoints (value component). The discernment that a specific
    standpoint may have broader implications for society/groups/individuals (in addition to those directly treated in the text), if society were organized according
    to Nozick’s standpoints (broader implication component). Regarding such a specific broader implication, the discernment that this broader implication can be
    explained by argument and conclusion (argument/conclusion component).

    Educationally critical differences (ecds): the argument/conclusion component

    Way of Understanding C
    “To scrutinize by paying attention to one broader implication of one of Nozick’s standpoints”
    The discernment of direct standpoints of Nozick’s view in line with the spirit of Nozick’s standpoints (standpoint component). The discernment of the main objective

    of the task as one of scrutinizing one/some of Nozick’s standpoints (scrutinizing component). The discernment of personal values/standpoints in relation to one/
    some of Nozick’s standpoints and offering opinions on it/them based on personal values/standpoints (value component). The discernment that a specific
    standpoint may have broader implications for society/groups/individuals (in addition to those directly treated in the text), if society were organized according
    to Nozick’s standpoints (broader implication component).

    Educationally critical differences (ecds): the broader implication component

    Way of Understanding B
    “To scrutinize one/some of Nozicḱs standpoints by using onés own values/standpoints”
    The discernment of direct standpoints of Nozick’s view in line with the spirit of Nozick’s standpoints (standpoint component). The discernment of the main objective

    of the task as one of scrutinizing one/some of Nozick’s standpoints (scrutinizing component). The discernment of personal values/standpoints in relation to one/
    some of Nozick’s standpoints and offering opinions on it/them based on personal values/standpoints (value component).

    Educationally critical differences (ecds): the scrutinizing component
    and the value component

    Way of Understanding A
    ”To describe some of Nozick’s standpoints”
    The discernment of direct standpoints of Nozick’s view in line with the spirit of Nozick’s standpoints (standpoint component). The discernment of the main objective

    of the task as one of describing some of Nozick’s standpoints (describing component).

    K. Larsson International Journal of Educational Research 84 (2017) 32–42

    37

    better for those who already have a lot. That he thinks it should be safe for everyone is also good. On the other hand he forgets
    everything else − how fair is it to be born rich and be able to live a good life without any effort while others are struggling and
    never succeed?” (S5)

    Way of understanding D
    Way of understanding D encompasses the same basic understanding as the previous way of understanding. But, in addition, Way

    of understanding D makes even more distinctions within understanding the scrutinizing part of the task, adding to the possibility of
    paying attention to one broader implication of one of Nozick’s standpoints, providing an opening for also explaining and drawing
    conclusions around such an implication. Apart from discerning the same components as Way of understanding C, Way of under-
    standing D then also discerns one additional component delimited as the argument/conclusion component. A typical answer from
    which this way of understanding was derived manifested critical thinking connected to all five components of the understanding. The
    extract below is a part of such an answer. The underlined segment in the answer shows where the argument/conclusion component
    was derived from.

    “… his idea that the State only exists to give people security so they won’t be afraid of someone stealing from them, does not
    include anything that is related to income support, assistance with standard of living, health-care, or the like/…/However, he
    forgets something very important − democracy. When people do not have food in their stomachs, they will not cope with
    engaging in the democratic and political process, and so democracy will fall. When your primary occupation is to take care of your
    children and find bread for tomorrow, there will be few who have the strength to vote or to get involved in party politics. That’s
    what I find to be the weakest with Robert Nozick’s arguments.” (S3)

    Way of understanding E
    Way of understanding E, the most complex way of understanding found within the student group, is characterised by the same

    understanding as Way of understanding D, but adds yet another distinction within understanding the scrutinizing part of the task.
    This distinction revolves around opening up for examining (at least two of) Nozick’s standpoints in relation to each other by looking
    for the possibility that there may be a contradiction between them. This distinction also poses a conceivable reorganisation of the way
    of discerning Nozick’s standpoints compared to all the previous ways of understanding the task (D-A). In the previous ways of
    understanding, there is a linear organization of the standpoints, where each standpoint is discerned as individual and discrete
    elements. In Way of understanding E, Nozick’s standpoints are instead (also) discerned as an integrated whole, where the standpoints
    can be checked against each other to test their internal congruence. Besides discerning the same components as Way of understanding
    D, Way of understanding E then also discerns one additional component delimited as the contradiction component. A typical answer
    from which this way of understanding was derived manifested critical thinking connected to all six components of the understanding.
    The extract below is an example of such an answer showing only the part of the answer from which the contradiction component was
    derived.

    “Robert thinks that it doesn’t matter how much each of us owns, it’s how they got it, i.e. if they purchased it legally, then it’s fair.
    But he also says that everyone’s got a right to life. But a life of extreme poverty is almost considered as no life. I think in this
    argumentation I can see a weakness in that he contradicts himself.” (S10)

    3. Using the empirical results together with phenomenographic learning and teaching theory

    Next I turn to a discussion on how the above phenomenographic empirical results can be used together with phenomenographic
    learning and teaching theory in educational situations to, in some respects, improve critical-thinking skills among students.

    Phenomenography views learning as a change in a person’s way of understanding a specific phenomenon (Marton & Booth, 1997).
    In an educational setting, this is usually viewed as a change in a person’s way of understanding the phenomenon in the direction of, or
    preferably in accordance with, the educational norm of understanding the phenomenon (Marton & Booth, 1997). What is to be
    learned builds on the hierarchically ordered ways of understanding a specific phenomenon and targets the differences between the
    various ways of understanding the phenomenon with the aim of obtaining access to what are called educationally critical differences
    or ecds (Marton & Booth, 1997). Each ecd is equivalent to a certain component and emerges at the intersection points between the
    different ways of understanding the phenomenon and against the background of the norm. More specifically, this means that an ecd
    emerges when two successive ways of understanding in the hierarchy are compared with each other. The ecd, or possibly the ecds, are
    determined and delimited by what is discerned in the more complex way of understanding and is missing in the less complex one and
    at the same time is a necessary condition to understand the phenomenon in line with the norm. When a deduction is made throughout
    the category system in order to find the ecds, you finally end up with a set of ecds, i.e. components. These ecds are what it is critical
    for a person who initially understands the phenomenon in the least complex way to discern in order to understand the phenomenon
    in accordance with the norm (Marton & Booth, 1997). If we apply this reasoning to the outcome space of the Nozick task, the ecds are:
    the scrutinizing component, the value component, the broader implication component, the argument/conclusion component and the
    contradiction component (see Table 1).

    If we now know the phenomenographic view on learning and the notions of educational norm and ecds, how then can a teacher
    give a person the opportunity to discern the ecds in relation to a norm of a certain phenomenon in an educational setting? According
    to the phenomenographic viewpoint, this can be done by using a set of different patterns of variation, the most important ones being
    called contrast and separation (Lo & Marton, 2012; Lo, 2012; Marton et al., 2004). The idea behind the pattern of variation called

    K. Larsson International Journal of Educational Research 84 (2017) 32–42
    38

    contrast is that a person will only be able to discern a particular ecd if they can at the same time discern what the ecd is not.
    Phenomenography says that a dimension of variation must be opened up in the teaching situation where the ecd constitutes one of at
    least two contrasting values within that dimension. Through a contrasting comparison between the ecd and the other value or values,
    it becomes possible to discern the specific distinctions of the ecd. Implicitly and simultaneously, it also becomes possible to discern
    both the distinctions of the other value or values that are brought into the dimension and the dimension in itself (Lo & Marton, 2012;
    Lo, 2012; Marton & Pang, 2006; Marton et al., 2004). Thus, in cases where several ecds could be said to belong to the same dimension
    of variation, all of them become possible to discern at the same time in the teaching situation (Lo, 2012). This is because they should
    all be brought in as contrasting values in the same dimension of variation.

    The idea of the pattern of variation separation is built on two basic assumptions. Firstly, if a specific ecd of a phenomenon is to be
    made discernible in a teaching situation, it must be separated from the phenomenon itself through the pattern of variation contrast.
    Secondly, when this happens, everything else concerning the phenomenon must be kept invariant or closed. In a teaching situation,
    this means that the ecds for the particular phenomenon belonging to the same dimension of variation are treated at the same time
    through the pattern of variation contrast, while ecds belonging to other dimensions of variation are kept invariant by their di-
    mensions of variation being kept closed. As a final remark, it is also important to be aware that although the two patterns − contrast
    and separation − are analytically separable, they are created and occur simultaneously in an educational situation (Lo & Marton,
    2012; Lo, 2012; Marton & Pang, 2006; Marton et al., 2004).

    Going back to the Nozick task, let’s see how the five ecds could be made possible to discern in an educational setting, i.e. giving
    the students an opportunity to improve their way of understanding the task, and in doing so, improving their critical thinking in
    relation to it.

    When phenomenography describes a teaching sequence in which the ecds for a phenomenon are made discernible, it normally
    follows a certain logic, meaning that if one ecd can be assumed to be the base for another, the former is presented before the latter. If
    this line of reasoning is followed in the case of the task, it could be argued that discerning the scrutinizing component is considered a
    base for discerning the other four ecds. This is because they are all different discernments of how to scrutinize Nozick’s standpoints
    and if one is to be able to make these different discernments, one first has to discern that the task is about scrutinizing, and not, for
    example, describing or reading out loud. It follows from this argument that the first ecd to be addressed in the suggested teaching
    sequence is the scrutinizing component.

    So how then do we go about giving the student the opportunity to discern this crucial component? The core of this component can
    be said to revolve around ways of handling one of Nozick’s standpoints, where discernment of the scrutinizing component is char-
    acterised by handling the standpoint by scrutinizing it. To give the students the opportunity to discern this component, it could be
    separated from its original task by opening a dimension of variation concerning [ways of handling one of Nozick’s standpoints] with
    one value consisting of the ecd [to scrutinize one of Nozick’s standpoints] and the contrasting value consisting of, for example, [to
    describe one of Nozick’s standpoints]. In an actual teaching situation, the opening of this dimension could be carried out by letting
    the students experience two different texts, one where a specific standpoint is described and one where the same standpoint is
    scrutinized. The emphasis in such a teaching situation should be to clarify the differences between scrutinizing a standpoint and
    describing a standpoint. The object of this is to specify, with the help of contrast, what distinguishes the scrutinizing component so it
    can be discerned by the students. As supposed above, it is crucial that both the chosen example texts are directed towards the same
    standpoint, so the separation does not become unclear by opening up a parallel dimension of variation concerning [Nozick’s
    standpoints].

    If we now have given the students the opportunity to discern the base-component how do we continue with the other ecds? As
    indicated above, it is possible to claim that the four remaining ecds all have one common core: they are all different types of
    scrutinizing one of Nozick’s standpoints (or two standpoints, in the case of the contradiction component). One way of offering the
    students the opportunity to discern all four of these critical differences, in terms of different types of scrutinizing, is for the teacher to
    open up the dimension of variation [ways to scrutinize one/two of Nozick’s standpoints] with the four different scrutinizing types as
    contrasting values within that dimension. In an actual teaching situation, this could be done in several ways. One way is to let the four
    different types of scrutinizing contrast with each other by means of comparing four different answers (to the task), each characterised
    by one of the four types. The emphasis of the comparison should be directed towards the unique distinction for each type of
    scrutinizing. Further, it is important that the presented answers concerning the value component, the broader implication component
    and the argument/conclusion component all are directed towards one of the two standpoints that are a part of the presented answer
    for the contradiction component. This is in order to minimize the risk of blurring the separation opening up a dimension of variation
    concerning [Nozick’s standpoints].

    When it comes to these four particular ecds, the opening of the dimension [ways of scrutinizing one/two of Nozick’s standpoints]
    is most likely not enough to allow the students to fully discern them. The teacher also has to address the organizational differences
    between the contradiction component (standpoint seen as an integrated whole containing the potential for scrutinizing the internal
    congruence of the standpoints) and the other three components (standpoint seen as linear and separated into parts, limiting the
    scrutinizing to one standpoint at the time). This could be done by the teacher opening up a dimension of variation in [ways of
    organizing Nozick’s standpoints] with the contrasting values [Nozick’s standpoints organized as integrated whole] and [Nozick’s
    standpoint organized as linear separate parts]. In an actual teaching situation, this could be done by choosing, for example, four of
    Nozick’s standpoints and organizing them against each other as an integrated whole. It is crucial that it is made clear that this way of
    organizing the standpoints is what distinguishes, and is a prerequisite for, the type of scrutiny that characterizes the contradiction
    component. This can then be contrasted in relation to an example where the same four standpoints are organized as linear and
    individual parts. As with the example concerning the “integrated whole” way of organizing, it is of equal importance that it is made

    K. Larsson International Journal of Educational Research 84 (2017) 32–42

    39

    evident that this way of organizing Nozick’s standpoints distinguishes the three other types of scrutinizing, and limits them to being
    directed towards just one standpoint at a time. To avoid opening a dimension of variation concerning [Nozick’s standpoints], making
    the separation fuzzy, it is important to underline that the same four standpoints should be used in both of the contrasting examples.

    We have now seen an example of a full teaching sequence where all of the ecds for the Nozick task are afforded to the students to
    discern. In other words, we have seen how phenomenographic results could be used together with phenomenographic learning and
    teaching theory to make it possible for the students to understand the task in line with the norm, i.e. in this case the most complex
    way found in the empirical data. As the most complex way of understanding the task delimits the most complex critical thinking in
    relation to the task, this also means that the students, by this sequence, would be given an opportunity to develop the most complex
    type of critical thinking in relation to the task.

    This being said, this possibility of improving the students’ critical thinking has to be seen as quite narrow, as the improvement is
    only in relation to the Nozick task. Phenomenography, however, does offer an opportunity to go beyond this limitation and use the
    empirical results from the Nozick task in a wider context of improving the students’ critical thinking.

    In this regard, let us first take a look at the phenomenographic notion about the possibility of general characteristics in the
    different content-specific ways of understanding a phenomenon. This notion rests on the assumption that if the content-specific ways
    of understanding a phenomenon are deprived of its specific content, there could be more general characteristics that might appear as
    components in ways of understanding another phenomenon of the same kind, or be generalised across several different phenomena of
    the same kind (Dahlgren, 1997[1984]; Wenestam, 1980). With this notion as a backbone, the scrutinizing component from the
    Nozick task can be taken as an illustrative example. The core of the content-specific scrutinizing component as it appears in the task
    could be articulated as ‘discerning that one of Nozick’s standpoints should be scrutinized’. If it is then content-deprived, losing its
    “Nozickness”, it could be articulated as ‘discerning that a standpoint should be scrutinized’. As such, it ought to be generalisable to
    other similar content consisting of one or more standpoints, preferably within the subject of civics.

    If we continue, using this reasoning together with phenomenographic learning and teaching theory, the content-deprived scru-
    tinizing component can be introduced to a new content consisting of one or more standpoints and again be made content-specific and
    discernible for students in relation to this new content by using the patterns of variation, contrast and separation. This could be done
    in the same manner as in the example I used above about how the scrutinizing component could be made discernible in relation to its
    original task. The only difference is that the content would now be something other then Nozick’s standpoints, for instance some
    political party’s standpoints on some issue. The dimension of variation that is opened in such a potential teaching situation could then
    be described in terms of [ways of handling one of the political party X’s standpoints on the issue Z] with one value being the, now
    once more content-specific, scrutinizing component [to scrutinize one of the political party X’s standpoints on issue Z] and the
    contrasting value being [to describe one of the political party X’s standpoints on issue Z].

    Without going any further, this kind of manoeuvre could be carried out for every ecd from the Nozick task, making each of them
    discernible in relation to new content consisting of one or more standpoints. In this way, the empirically derived ecds from the Nozick
    task could be used in an educational situation to give students the opportunity to improve their critical thinking in a wider sense.

    4. Concluding remarks

    The article has focused on what has been called the theory issue. It has introduced the phenomenographic approach to the critical
    thinking scene and shown how this approach can be used as a strong theoretical frame both for understanding differences in
    manifestations of critical thinking and, based on such an understanding, designing learning experiences to develop students’ critical-
    thinking skills. As such, the article provides an alternative to the (scarce) theoretical framing that has been tried so far. In relation to
    the approaches used by Kuhn and by King and Kitchener, phenomenography offers an alternative to their cognitive developmental
    tradition, by bringing in the experiential tradition, with roots in phenomenology and hermeneutics. Most importantly, phenomen-
    ography offers a teaching theory to go along with the learning theory, something that is more or less absent in Kuhn’s, as well as King
    and Kitchener’s, theoretical framing (King & Kitchener, 1994, 2004; Kuhn & Dean, 2004; Kuhn, 1991, 1999). Their perspective fo-
    cuses on a theoretical description of the learning, while not giving any clear-cut and detailed theoretical description of the teaching
    that makes that learning possible. For example, Kuhn (Kuhn & Dean 2004) does not give any real theoretical description of the
    teaching in relation to changing students’ mental model for causality to a more complex one with regard to the why feature. She only
    describes the learning (in theoretical terms) that needs to take place for such a change, i.e. the shifting of mental models. Phe-
    nomenography can, on the other hand, by means of, for instance, the theory about patterns of variation and how they should be used
    in teaching, present a teaching theory to go along with the learning theory. In this way, phenomenography does not simply provide
    another approach, but offers a distinctly new theoretical aspect to the field, which has a substantial bearing on what McMillan has
    asked for, i.e. a “theoretical description of the nature of the learning experience” that would help to develop critical thinking
    (McMillan, 1987:14). From a more general perspective, the article, through its use of phenomenography, addresses the overall lack of
    theory within the field and so makes a valuable contribution to the development of this crucial area.

    Though the article has shown how phenomenography could act as a solid and cohesive theoretical frame for understanding and
    teaching critical thinking, it is important to notice that the study the article uses to introduce the phenomenographic approach is just
    an example of how the approach can be used. Should phenomenography be used in relation to other types of contents, subjects or
    problems, or if another definition of critical thinking is to be used, appropriate adjustments must be made. And, as stated earlier,
    when it comes to the ecds and the teaching sequences emanating from the Nozick task in the example study, they are, when stripped
    of their Nozickness, to some extent applicable to other contents and subjects where one or more standpoints are to be scrutinized.
    However, there is no theoretical ground to go further than that.

    K. Larsson International Journal of Educational Research 84 (2017) 32–42

    40

    In sum, the article confirms that phenomenography seems to provide a feasible way to grapple with the theory issue and gives a
    strong foundation for developing the theoretical aspects of understanding and teaching critical thinking. Even so, to more thoroughly
    examine the feasibility of using phenomenography, further exploration of the approach within this domain of research is necessary.
    This can be done in several ways. One of the studies which most urgently needs to be conducted is to test the teaching sequences
    suggested above in real classroom situations and evaluate their success. This would presumably be best carried out with a researcher
    leading and evaluating the project, and teachers (who would receive instruction in phenomenographic theory) conducting the actual
    teaching sequences. Other projects to test the feasibility of the phenomenographic approach could aim at finding ecds emanating
    from other types of contents, subjects and problems, and design, test and evaluate teaching sequences based on these investigations.
    Such efforts could be made in the action-research inspired learning study practice, which has evolved within the phenomenographic
    tradition (Pang & Ki 2016; Pang & Lo 2012; Pang & Marton 2003). In this kind of research, one or more researchers work together
    with a group of teachers to develop a specified skill among a group of students using phenomenographic theory as a base.

    As cited at the beginning of the article, Kuhn and Dean described critical thinking as one of the “major unsolved problems of
    pedagogy” (2004:269). It is not reasonable to think that one single research endeavour or one specific theoretical approach could
    solve this problem. This article, however, argues that the phenomenographic approach could be one path worth further exploring on
    such a quest.

    Appendix A. Text on Robert Nozick

    Robert Nozick
    In his book “Anarchy, State and Utopia”, the philosopher Robert Nozick writes that we always have the right to keep what we

    acquired legally. If everybody originally acquired their property in a just way − for example, by inheritance, gift, purchase or
    exchange − then the distribution that arises in a society is also fair.

    According to Nozick, a state may never go in and redistribute by taxing the rich and giving to those who are worse off. It is unfair,
    even if it were true that a small group owns everything and the majority are starving.

    The state should only guarantee that people can live safely and in security, and that they are not subjected to violence or anyone
    trying to steal their property. Nozick calls such a state the minimal state.

    By far the most important rights a man has, according to Nozick, is the right to property and the right to life. The state may never
    restrict these rights. Then, what the allocation of resources looks like in practice is irrelevant from an equity point of view, that is, as
    long as everyone has acquired what they own in a just way. On the other hand, if someone acquired their property incorrectly − for
    example, by threats or theft − it must be corrected, no matter how far back in time the error was made.

    (The text is a modified version of a text from the book “Dilemma – om rätt och fel, etik och moral” [“Dilemma – right and wrong,
    ethics and moral”] by Uriel Hedengren. Stockholm: Natur & Kultur, 2002.)

    Appendix B. Distribution of individuals across categories

    Way of understanding A: S1, S10, S14, S19
    Way of understanding B: S3, S6, S7, S11, S13
    Way of understanding C: S5
    Way of understanding D: S2, S8, S9, S12, S15, S17, S18
    Way of understanding E: S4
    Not categorised: S16 (due to absence of answer)

    References

    Behar-Horenstein, L., & Niu, L. (2011). Teaching critical thinking skills in higher education: A review of the literature. Journal of College Teaching & Learning,
    8(February (2)), 25–

    41

    .

    Beyer, B. K. (1995). Critical thinking. Bloomington, Indiana: The Phi Delta Kappa Educational Foundation.
    Booth, S., & Ingerman (2002). Making sense of Physics in the first year of study. Learning and Instruction, 12(2002), 493–507.
    Brodin, E. (2007). Critical thinking in scholarship. Meanings, conditions and development. Lund: Lund University. Department of Education.
    Brunt, A. (2005). Critical thinking in nursing: An integrated review. The Journal of Continuing Education in Nursing, 36(March/April (2)), 60–67.
    Chowning, J., Griswold, J., Kovarik, D., & Collins, L. (2012). Fostering critical thinking, reasoning, and argumentation skills through bioethics education. PLoS One,

    7(May (5)).
    Dahlgren L-O. (1997) [1984]. Learning Conceptions and Outcomes. In F. Marton, D. Hounsell, & N. Entwistle (ed.) The Experience of Learning. Edinburgh: Scottish

    Academic Press Limited.
    Ennis, R. H. (1987). A taxonomy of critical thinking dispositions and abilities. In J. K. Baron, & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Teaching thinking skills: Theory and practice (pp. 9–

    26). .
    Ennis, R. H. (1993). Critical thinking assessment. Theory into Practice, 32(Summer (3)), 179–186.
    Facione, P. A. (1990). Critical thinking: A statement of expert consensus for purposes of educational assessment and instruction. Executive Summary The Delphi ReportMillbrae:

    The California Academic Press.
    Facione, P. A. (2006). Critical thinking: What is it and why it counts. California: Academic Press.
    Fung, D. (2014). Promoting critical thinking through effective group work: A teaching intervention for Hong Kong primary school students. International Journal of

    Educational Research, 66, 45–62.
    Johnson, R. H., & Hamby, B. (2015). A meta-Level approach to the problem of defining ‘Critical thinking’. Argumentation, 4, 417–430.
    King, P. M., & Kitchener, K. S. (1994). Developing reflective judgment. Understanding and promoting intellectual growth and critical thinking in adolescents and adults. San

    Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers.
    King, P. M., & Kitchener, K. S. (2004). Reflective judgment: Theory and research on the development of epistemic assumptions through adulthood. Educational

    K. Larsson International Journal of Educational Research 84 (2017) 32–42
    41

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0030

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0030

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0075

    Psychologist, 39(1), 5–18.
    Kuhn, D., & Dean, D., Jr. (2004). Metacognition: A bridge between cognitive psychology and educational practice. Theory Into Practice, 43(4), 268–273.
    Kuhn, D. (1991). The skills of argument. New York: Cambridge University Press.
    Kuhn, D. (1999). A developmental model of critical thinking. Educational Researcher, 28(2), 16–46.
    Larsson K. (in press) Critical thinking in students’ ethical reasoning: A reflection on some examples from the Swedish national tests in religious education. In O. Franck

    (ed.) Assessment in Ethics Education −A Case of National Tests in Religious Education. Dordrecht: Springer.
    Lo, M. L., & Marton, F. (2012). Towards a science of the art of teaching: Using variation theory as a guiding principle of pedagogical design. International Journal for

    Lesson and Learning Studies, 1(1), 7–22.
    Lo, M. L. (2012). Variation theory and the improvement of teaching and learning. Göteborg: Acta Universitatis Gothoburgensis.
    Martin, D. S. (2005). Critical thinking for democracy and social justice. In N. M. Michelli, & D. L. Keiser (Eds.), Teacher education for democracy and social justice (pp.

    209–228). New York: Routledge.
    Marton, F., & Booth, S. (1997). Learning and awareness. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    Marton, F., & Pang, M. F. (2006). On some necessary conditions of learning. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 15(2), 193–220.
    Marton F., Säljö R. (1997) [1984]. Approaches to Learning. In F. Marton, D. Hounsell, & N. Entwistle (eds.) The Experience of Learning. Edinburgh: Scottish Academic

    Press Limited.
    Marton, F., Runesson, U., & Tsui, A. B. M. (2004). The space of learning. In F. Marton, & A. B. M. Tsui (Eds.), Classroom discourse and the space of learning (pp. 3–40).

    Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
    Marton, F. (1981). Phenomenography −Describing conceptions of the world around us. Instructional Science, 10, 177–200.
    Marton, F. (1996). Cognosco ergo sum −Reflections on reflections. In G. Dall’Alba, & B. Hasselgren (Eds.), Reflections on phenomenography. Toward a methodology? (pp.

    163–187). Göteborg: Acta Universitatis Gothoburgensis.
    Mason, M. (2007). Critical thinking and learning. Educational Philosophy and Theory, 39(4), 339–349.
    McMillan, J. H. (1987). Enhancing college students’ critical thinking: A review of studies. Research in Higher Education, 26(1), 3–29.
    McPeck, J. E. (1990a). Teaching critical thinking. Dialogue and dialectic. New York: Routledge.
    McPeck, J. E. (1990b). Critical thinking and subject specificity: A reply to ennis. Educational Researcher, 19(4), 10–12.
    Menssen, S. (1993). Critical thinking and the construction of knowledge. The American Behavioral Scientist, 37(1), 85–93.
    Moore, T. (2013). Critical thinking: Seven definitions in search of a concept. Studies in Higher Education, 38(4), 506–522.
    Newmann, F. M. (1991). Classroom thoughtfulness and students’ higher order thinking: Common indicators and diverse social studies courses. Madison, Wisconsin: National

    Center on Effective Secondary Schools.
    Norris, S. P., Leighton, J. P., & Phillips, L. M. (2004). What is at stake in knowing the content and capabilities of children’s minds? A case for basing high stakes tests on

    cognitive models, 2. Theory and Research in Education, 2(3), 283–308.
    Norris, S. P. (1985). Synthesis of research on critical thinking. Educational Leadership, 1985(May), 40–45.
    Ojo, E., & Booth, S. (2009). Internationalisation of higher education in a South African university: A phenomenographic study of students’ conceptions. Education as

    Change, 13(December (2)), 309–323.
    Pang, M., & Ki, W. (2016). Revisiting the idea of critical aspects. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 1–14.
    Pang, M., & Lo, M. L. (2012). Learning study: Helping teachers to use theory, develop professionally, and produce new knowledge to be shared. Instructional Science,

    40(3), 589–606.
    Pang, M., & Marton, F. (2003). Beyond lesson study: Comparing two ways of facilitating the grasp of some economic concepts. Instructional Science, 31(3), 175–194.
    Paul, R. (1992). Critical thinking: What, why, and how. New Directions for Community Colleges, 1992(77), 3–24.
    Petress, K. (2004). Critical thinking: An extended definition. Education, 124(3), 461–466.
    Phillips, V., & Bond, C. (2004). Undergraduates’ experiences of critical thinking. Higher Education Research & Development, 23(August (3)), 277–294.
    Sandberg, J. (1994). Human competence at work. An interpretative approach. Göteborg: BAS.
    Sandberg, J. (1996). Are phenomenographic results reliable? In G. Dall’Alba, & B. Hasselgren (Eds.), Reflections on phenomenography. Toward a methodology? (pp. 129–

    140). Göteborg: Acta Universitatis Gothoburgensis.
    Sendag, S., & Ferhan Odabasi, H. (2009). Effects of an online problem based learning course on content knowledge acquisition and critical thinking skills.

    Computers & Education, 53(1), 132–141.
    Svensson L. (1997) [1984]. Skills in learning and Organising Knowledge. In F. Marton, D. Hounsell, N. Entwistle (eds.) The Experience of Learning. Edinburgh: Scottish

    Academic Press Limited.
    Tsui, L. (1998). A review of research on critical thinking. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the association for study of higher education, 5–8 november 1998.
    Tsui, L. (2002). Fostering critical thinking through effective pedagogy: Evidence from four institutional case studies. The Journal of Higher Education, 73(6), 740–763.
    Tsui, L. (2007). Cultivating critical thinking: Insights from an elite liberal arts college. The Journal of General Education, 56(3 & 4), 200–227.
    Wenestam, C.-G. (1980). Qualitative differences in retention. Göteborg: Acta Universitatis Gothoburgensis.
    Wheeler, & Collins (2003). The influence of concept mapping on critical thinking in baccalaureate nursing students. Journal of Professional Nursing, 19(6), 339–346.

    K. Larsson International Journal of Educational Research 84 (2017) 32–42
    42

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0085

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0120

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0883-0355(16)31257-5/sbref0265

      Understanding and teaching critical thinking—A new approach
      Introduction
      The empirical study
      Aim
      Method
      Definition of critical thinking
      The task
      Task design
      Participants
      Implementation
      Empirical analysis
      Empirical results
      Using the empirical results together with phenomenographic learning and teaching theory
      Concluding remarks
      Text on Robert Nozick
      Distribution of individuals across categories
      References

    lable at ScienceDirect

    Learning and Instruction 29 (2014) 31e42

    Contents lists avai

    Learning and Instruction

    journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/learninstruc

    Improving critical thinking: Effects of dispositions and instructions
    on economics students’ reasoning skills

    Anita Heijltjes a,*, Tamara van Gog b, Jimmie Leppink c, Fred Paas b,d

    a Learning and Innovation Centre, Avans University of Applied Sciences, Hogeschoollaan 1, Postbus 90116, 4800RA Breda, The Netherlands
    b Institute of Psychology, Erasmus University Rotterdam, The Netherlands
    c Department of Educational Development and Research, Maastricht University, The Netherlands
    d Interdisciplinary Educational Research Institute, University of Wollongong, Australia

    a r t i c l e i n f o

    Article history:
    Received 15 February 2012
    Received in revised form
    10 July 2013
    Accepted 17 July 2013

    Keywords:
    Critical thinking instructions
    Dispositions
    Biased reasoning

    * Corresponding author.
    E-mail address: aeg.heijltjes@avans.nl (A. Heijltjes

    0959-4752/$ e see front matter � 2013 Elsevier Ltd.
    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.07.003

    a b s t r a c t

    This experiment investigated the impact of critical thinking dispositions and instructions on economics
    students’ performance on reasoning skills. Participants (N ¼ 183) were exposed to one of four conditions:
    critical thinking instruction, critical thinking instruction with self-explanation prompts during subse

    quent practice, critical thinking instruction with activation prompts during subsequent practice, or no
    critical thinking instruction or prompts (control). In all conditions, practice was a within-subjects factor,
    some task categories present in the test were practiced on a business case, others were not. Participants
    in the instruction conditions significantly outperformed participants in the control condition on th

    e

    immediate and delayed post-test, but only on the practiced task categories e with the exception of the
    self-explanations condition, which also showed a better performance than the control condition on not-
    practiced categories, though only on the immediate post-test. Dispositions (i.e., Actively Open-minded
    Thinking and Need for Cognition) predicted reasoning skills at pre-test but did not interact with in-
    structions on post-tests performances.

    � 2013 Elsevier Ltd.

    All rights reserved.

    1. Introduction

    Against the background of complex and rapidly changing busi-
    ness environments, economics students are expected to become
    critical thinkers (Klebba & Hamilton, 2007; Smith, 2003). Critical
    thinking enables students to make sound logical and unbiased de-
    cisions, and in educational situations it has been shown to lead to
    better learning and transfer outcomes (e.g., Facione, 2009; Halpern,
    1998; Helsdingen, Van Gog, & Van Merriënboer, 2011). Therefore, it
    is surprising that critical thinking is rarely explicitly taught in eco-
    nomics curricula (Jones, 2007). This might be due to the prevailing
    view that immersion in business methods and strategies will lead to
    the spontaneous development of critical thinking skills (Jones, 2007

    ).

    However, there is little evidence that such skills develop spontane-
    ously as a consequence of instruction in a discipline (Halpern, 1999).
    On the contrary, research has shown that critical thinking seems to
    increase only if taught explicitly (e.g., Abrami et al., 2008). It is
    difficult, though, for educators in economics (and other domains for

    ).
    All rights reserved.

    that matter) to derive guidelines from existing research regarding
    when, where and how to foster critical thinking throughout the
    curriculum (Smith, 2003). According to Abrami et al. (2008), existing
    studies on critical thinking instruction often lack a powerful empir-
    ical design (see also McMillan, 1987; Ten Dam & Volman, 2004;
    Wolcott, Baril, Cunningham, Fordham, & St.Pierre, 2002). Moreover,
    the definition of critical thinking or the aspects of critical thinking
    that are investigated tend to differ (Abrami et al., 2008).

    The present studyaims to empiricallyexamine the effects of explicit
    critical thinking instructions on an essential aspect of critical thinking,
    whichis highly important todecision-making incomplex andhigh-risk
    situations that often occur in the field of economics: the avoidance of
    biased reasoning by means of more controlled rational judgment and
    decision-making (Evans, 2003; Smith, 2003; Stanovich & Stanovich,
    2010; Tversky & Kahneman, 1983; West, Toplak, & Stanovich, 2008).
    Because such biases seem inherent to human reasoning and decision-
    making, economists are no exception and are also prone to violating
    the principles of rationality (Sanfey, Loewenstein, McClure, & Cohen,
    2006). This study investigates whether declarative critical thinking in-
    struction followed by practice, either by itself or combined with self-
    explanation or activation prompts during practice, would enhance

    Delta:1_’

    Delta:1_given name

    Delta:1_surname

    Delta:1_given name

    mailto:aeg.heijltjes@avans.nl

    http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.07.003&domain=pdf

    www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/09594752

    http://www.elsevier.com/locate/learninstruc

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.07.003

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.07.003

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.07.003

    A. Heijltjes et al. / Learning and Instruction 29 (2014) 31e4232

    economics students’ learning of reasoning skills. Moreover, it is inves-
    tigated whether individual differences in thinking dispositions are
    associated with initial performance on those skills and with learning.

    Before describing the research on critical thinking instructions
    and the rationale behind our study, we will first address the prin-
    ciples of critical thinking (especially in relation to biased reasoning)
    and its underlying cognitive mechanisms in more detail.

    1.1. Critical thinking

    The ‘American Philosophical Association Delphi Panel’ (Facione,
    1990), characterized critical thinking for educational purposes (i.e.,
    to support the instruction and assessment of critical thinking) as an
    extensive concept including both cognitive skills and dispositions.
    A critical thinker must, for example, be skilled at reasoning, which
    refers to the cognitive process of drawing conclusions from given
    information (Facione, 1990). Although the critical thinking litera-
    ture in educational research has focused on a wide variety of topics
    in the past decades (see e.g. Angeli & Valanides, 2009), little
    attention has been paid to the avoidance of biased reasoning (West
    et al., 2008). Critical thinking, in terms of avoiding biased reasoning,
    may be classified as a subspecies of rational thinking (Facione,
    2009; Stanovich, 2011; West et al., 2008). The dual processing
    framework unraveled the underlying cognitive processes of
    rational thinking (e.g., Evans, 2003, 2008, 2011). According to the-
    ories of dual processing, two distinct types of reasoning processes
    are at work. Type 1 processes have a rapid, automatic nature and
    involve little reflection. Decision-making is based on past experi-
    ences and requires little effort, which is useful and efficient in many
    routine situations. However, because of its automaticity, it might
    also result in biased thinking in other situations, unless Type 2
    processes overrule these automatic responses by explicit reasoning
    efforts. Type 2 processes are slow, sequential in nature, and require
    the exclusion of attention to other matters, and therefore draw
    more heavily on working memory capacity.

    Stanovich (2009) distinguishes Type 2 processes into reflective
    and algorithmic operations. The reflective mind operates at an
    intentional level based on dispositions such as beliefs, cognitive
    style, goals, and epistemic values, which affect the algorithmic
    mind. Research has shown that dispositions such as ‘actively open-
    minded thinking’ (AOT, i.e., the active search for evidence against
    one’s own beliefs, plans, or goals and the ability to weigh available
    evidence fairly; Baron, 2008) and ‘need for cognition’ (NFC, i.e., “the
    tendency for an individual to engage in and enjoy thinking”;
    Cacioppo, Petty, & Kao, 1984, p. 306) predict performance on tasks
    associated with rational thinking such as syllogisms, statistical
    reasoning, and framing (Stanovich & West, 1997, 2007; for a review
    on NFC, see Cacioppo, Petty, Feinstein, & Jarvis, 1996). Individuals
    with higher AOT scores performed better on argument evaluation
    (Stanovich & West, 1997) and co-variation judgment (Sa, Kelley, Ho,
    & Stanovich, 2005; West et al., 2008), considered more alternative
    possibilities (i.e., other possibilities than initially favored), asked
    themselves more frequently what possibility would produce the
    best expected outcomes, and showed less overconfidence in hasty
    conclusions than individuals with lower AOT scores (Baron, 2008).
    NFC is characterized by cognitive motivation that predicts perfor-
    mance on cognitive tasks. For example, individuals with higher NFC
    scores have been found to recall more of the information to which
    they are exposed, are more focused on substantive merits of the
    information (e.g., are more influenced by the quality of arguments
    of persuasive messages), generate more task-relevant thoughts
    which reflect the quality of arguments, make more thoughtful
    judgments (i.e., scrutinize and elaborate material more) and put
    more effort into cognitive tasks than individuals with lower NFC
    scores.

    The algorithmic mind performs analytic and inhibitory processes
    that enable a person to process information in such a way that the
    correct actions are taken (Stanovich, 2009). Thus, the algorithmic
    mind has the ability to override Type 1 processes by applying
    knowledge of inferential rules and strategies of rational thought (e.g.
    probabilistic reasoning, causal reasoning and logic). Failures to
    override Type 1 reasoning often occur on classical heuristics and
    biases tasks, which tend to evoke an automatic response, while they
    require causal and probabilistic reasoning, assessment of covaria-
    tion, a tendency to think statistically, and to think of alternative
    explanations. Forexample Tverskyand Kahneman (1983) illustrated
    poor probabilistic reasoning on a classical conjuntion task: “Linda is
    31 years old, single, outspoken, and very bright. She majored in phi-
    losophy. As a student, she was deeply concerned with issues of
    discrimination and social justice, and also participated in anti-nuclear
    demonstrations. Which of the two alternatives are more probable: 1.
    Linda is a bank-teller or 2. Linda is a bank teller and is active in the
    feminist movement.” (p. 297). Most people are inclined to choose
    option 2, but indicating that option 2 is more probable than option 1
    violates the conjunction rule because a conjunction cannot be more
    probable than one of its ingredients (P(A&B) � P (B)).

    Although heuristics and biases tasks are largely unexploited in
    the traditional critical thinking literature and measurement (West
    et al., 2008), these tasks allow for the assessment of the degree of
    rationality in terms of reflective and algorithmic mechanisms
    (Stanovich, Toplak, & West, 2008). Failures to override Type 1 pro-
    cesses, demonstrated on heuristics and biases tasks (Stanovich,
    2009), might occur due to lack of declarative knowledge of
    reasoning skills or insufficient strategies to use available knowledge.
    Both might presumably be counteracted by instructions.

    1.2. Critical thinking instruction: avoiding biased reasoning

    Relatively few studies have focused on the questions of how to
    avoid biased reasoning in favor of more rational thinking
    (Stanovich & Stanovich, 2010), of whether rational thinking is
    trainable, of what type of instructions would be most helpful, and
    of whether instructions have a persistent impact on learners’
    thinking beyond the period of instruction (for reviews see Ritchart
    & Perkins, 2005; Stanovich, 2011).

    Research that has been conducted on educational interventions
    revealed some evidence that explicit rule training on the law of
    large numbers fostered performance on base-rate problems (Fong,
    Krantz, & Nisbett, 1986), that explicit debiasing instructions (i.e.,
    instructions to decouple prior beliefs and opinions from evaluation
    of evidence and arguments such as prompting subjects with ‘give
    both reasons for and reasons against’) improved performance on
    argument generation tasks and syllogistic reasoning tasks
    (Macpherson & Stanovich, 2007). Fong et al. (1986) found that both
    teaching the rules and guidance to apply these rules in particular
    domains by examples, were both effective; however, providing
    examples significantly improved the use of abstract rule systems.
    According to Nisbett, Fong, Lehman, and Cheng (1987), on some
    tasks (e.g. causal and conditional tasks) reasoning improvement
    only had an enduring effect if abstract rule training and example
    training were both provided.

    In the academic domains of psychology, medicine, and law,
    Lehman, Lempert, and Nisbett (1988) showed that teaching infer-
    ential and logical rules improved students’ reasoning performances
    in domain-related tasks as well as on everyday life problems. In the
    financial domain, Larrick, Morgan, and Nisbett (1990) found that
    participants who had been instructed on cost-benefit principles,
    followed by using these principles on examples and indicating
    whether they agreed or disagreed with the reasoning in the ex-
    amples, became more rational on reasoning (i.e., responded more

    A. Heijltjes et al. / Learning and Instruction 29 (2014) 31e42 33

    normatively) compared to untrained participants and applied
    normative principles on a different type of problem, both imme-
    diately and after a full month, and in different contexts (i.e., transfer
    took place).

    These findings on debiasing instructions are consistent with
    studies in the broader critical thinking literature (e.g. Abrami et al.,
    2008; Angeli & Valanides, 2009; Bangert-Drowns & Bankert, 1990),
    which show that general critical thinking instruction combined
    with the integration of critical thinking principles into subject
    matter instruction (e.g., thought provoking activities) works best
    for improving critical thinking.

    Moreover, these findings suggest that two key-factors appear to
    play a role in critical thinking instruction: explicit teaching of
    reasoning principles and room for practicing these principles. Ac-
    cording to Bransford, Sherwood, Vye, and Rieser (1986), combining
    explicit instructions with practice allows students to integrate
    general rules into a domain specific context, which helps them to
    integrate, combine, and cluster information in a meaningful way.
    This would result in a conceptual and practical organization of
    knowledge that facilitates future performance on such tasks.

    When both explicit instruction and practicing would be
    required for acquiring reasoning skills, then the question arises
    whether additional prompts during practice might have added
    value and if so, which kind of prompts? Two potential prompting
    methods might be useful and will be elaborated upon below: self-
    explanation and activation prompts.

    1.2.1. Self-explanation prompts
    Regarding self-explanation, Austin, Gregory, and Chiu (2008)

    found that prompting students to self-assess and reflect
    improved their performances on reasoning tasks. Participants who
    provided a brief written explanation of how they arrived at the
    rating and a rationalization of why they selected a particular
    answer, demonstrated a significantly better performance on critical
    thinking (i.e., less heuristic and more algorithmic reasoning), than
    participants who did not receive these prompts. Angeli and
    Valanides (2009) examined the impact of critical thinking teach-
    ing methods in performances on an ill-defined problem (e.g. to
    discuss an issue and to produce an outline for a paper) and found
    the highest effect size for the condition in which students discussed
    an issue, and reflected on their thinking combined with a short
    lecture about critical thinking. These findings are consistent with
    studies about the effectiveness of self-explanation.

    Self-explanation fosters the proper use of available knowledge
    and skills, which engages students in active and meaningful
    learning while effectively monitoring their understanding (Roy &
    Chi, 2005). The underlying cognitive mechanisms of self-
    explaining have been described as “generating inferences to fill
    missing information, integrating information within study mate-
    rials, integrating new information with prior knowledge and
    monitoring and repairing faulty knowledge” (Roy & Chi, 2005, p.
    272). According to Lombrozo (2006), explaining is especially useful
    for approaches to reasoning as it promotes learning by the inte-
    gration of new information with prior beliefs. Explanations help to
    assess the probability of for example claims in light of prior beliefs,
    evoke beliefs-revision, encourage learners to re-describe materials,
    and allow them to identify relevant principles (Williams &
    Lombrozo, 2010).

    Moreover, self-explanation fosters generalizations. Aleven and
    Koedinger (2002) found that students who explained their steps
    during problem solving learned more effectively and generalize
    readily to novel situations, than students who did not explain their
    steps. Hence self-explanation might be a particularly effective
    strategy to promote both learning and transfer (e.g., Lombrozo,
    2006; Renkl, 2005; Rittle-Johnson, 2006), even at a delay: Rittle-

    Johnson (2006) found that the effects of direct instruction com-
    bined with self-explanation facilitated learning and transfer addi-
    tively and that the benefits persisted over a 2-week delay. Finally,
    these beneficial effects of self-explanations occur even though
    participants rarely receive feedback on the quality of their expla-
    nations (Matthews & Rittle-Johnson, 2009). Based on these studies
    it can be hypothesized that prompting self-explanation during
    practice of reasoning tasks would have an additional impact on
    acquisition of reasoning skills, as it helps not only to integrate ac-
    quired knowledge and skills (which only practising would also do),
    but helps to examine gaps, evoke belief-revision, and foster gen-
    eralizations during practice.

    1.2.2. Activation prompts
    Activation prompts relate to the assumption that reasoning

    biases are due to a inhibition failure in working memory and not to
    a lack of understanding of probabilistic principles or a lack of
    deductive competence (Moutier, Angeard, & Houdé, 2002; Moutier
    & Houdé, 2003). Moutier et al. showed that inhibition training led
    to better performance on Wason selection tasks and conjunction
    tasks. For instance, they used an adaptation of the classical ‘Linda
    task’ described above, which requires inhibition of the misleading
    scheme (the conjunction) and activation of the relevant scheme
    (the probability). A control group (no training) was compared to a
    strictly logical training group (in which the experimenter explained
    the misleading scheme and admonished subjects not to fall in a
    trap), and an inhibition-training group. The inhibition-training
    group received the logical training and in addition, participants
    were encouraged to redirect their attention toward logically rele-
    vant information. However, even though it can be effective, inhi-
    bition training may have the unintended side effect of sensitizing
    the mind to other thoughts that have to be avoided (Wenzlaff &
    Bates, 2000). Stanovich and Stanovich (2010) also stated that sup-
    pressing the initial response (Type 1 reasoning) is only helpful
    when a better response is available to substitute for it. In line with
    findings on inhibition training, an alternative means of increasing
    Type 2 reasoning might be the activation of the ‘weaker’ cues
    instead of the suppression of dominant ones (i.e., evoke an atten-
    tion shift from irrelevant to relevant information; Miyake,
    Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, & Howerter, 2000).

    Thus, based on previous studies it would be reasonable to expect
    that activation prompts during practice would encourage an
    attention shift to relevant tasks aspects, which would have an
    additional impact on the acquisition of reasoning skills.

    In sum, an important question for education, which is addressed
    in the present study, is whether critical thinking in terms of unbi-
    ased reasoning can be enhanced by explicit instructions in combi-
    nation with practice, and whether self-explanation or the
    activation prompts during practice can further enhance reasoning
    skills.

    1.3. The present study

    The present study addresses the following questions: a) What is
    the impact of individual differences in dispositions on economics
    students’ critical thinking as measured by their performance on
    judgment and reasoning tasks? b) What are the effects of in-
    structions, practice, and prompts during practice on economics
    students’ critical thinking as measured by their performance on
    reasoning tasks immediately and after a three-week delay?

    Regarding dispositions, we hypothesized in line with previous
    research that those participants with higher scores on dispositions
    (i.e., AOT and NFC) would score better on an initial assessment of
    critical thinking skills than participants with lower scores on these
    dispositions (Hypothesis 1). An interesting related question is

    A. Heijltjes et al. / Learning and Instruction 29 (2014) 31e4234

    whether students with higher scores on dispositions would also
    benefit more from the critical instruction than students with
    lower scores on dispositions, or whether instruction is equally
    effective for all participants; this question is explored here
    (Question 1).

    Regarding instructions, it is hypothesized that critical thinking
    instruction enhances performance on critical thinking skills
    compared to no instruction, both immediately (Hypothesis 2a) and
    after a three-week delay (Hypothesis 2b); however, based on prior
    research it can be expected that this beneficial effect would only
    arise when instruction is combined with practice. Therefore,
    practice is taken into account as a within-subjects factor, and it is
    hypothesized that only those tasks which will be instructed and
    practiced, would enhance reasoning skills (Hypothesis 2c).

    If critical thinking instruction has a general effect, then perfor-
    mance on both practiced and not-practiced tasks should be
    enhanced compared to the control (i.e., no instruction) condition;
    however, if instruction only has a beneficial effect when combined
    with practice, only performance on practiced tasks should be
    enhanced compared to the control condition. Secondly, it is hy-
    pothesized that after instruction, combining practice with prompts
    (either self-explanation or activation prompts) would foster
    acquisition of critical thinking skills compared to instruction and
    practice only (i.e., without prompts) both immediately (Hypothesis
    3a) and after a three-week delay (Hypothesis 3b).

    2. Method

    2.1. Participants and design

    Participants were 195 part-time Economics students of a Dutch
    University of Applied Sciences with various educational back-
    grounds (in the Netherlands, Universities of Applied Sciences can
    be entered via various secondary education trajectories). Twelve
    participants had to be excluded because of missing data, leaving a
    final sample of 183 students (121 men, 62 women; age M ¼ 29.3,
    SD ¼ 6.5). Participants were randomly assigned to one of four
    conditions: 1) no critical thinking instruction (control; n ¼ 40), 2)
    critical thinking instruction (n ¼ 46), 3) critical thinking instruc-
    tion plus self-explanation prompts (n ¼ 47), and 4) critical thinking
    instruction plus activation prompts (n ¼ 50). The experiment
    consisted of 5 phases: 1) pre-test, 2) instructions (critical thinking
    or unrelated topic depending on condition), 3) practice of
    reasoning skills on a business case, 4) immediate post-test, 5)
    delayed post-test. Note regarding phase 3 that in each condition,
    practice was a within-subjects factor (i.e., some task categories
    were practiced during the practice phase, others were not). In the
    prompting conditions, the prompts were provided during practice
    (phase 3).

    2.2. Materials and procedure

    2.2.1. Critical thinking disposition tests
    We used a Dutch translation of the 41-item Actively Open-

    minded Thinking test (AOT; Stanovich & West, 2007) and the
    18-item (short form) Need for Cognition questionnaire (NFC;
    Cacioppo et al., 1984) to measure critical thinking dispositions.
    Both consist of items requiring a response on a 6-point Likert scale
    (AOT: as in Stanovich & West, 2007; NFC: as in West et al., 2008;
    note that Cacioppo et al., 1984 used a 9-point scale) ranging from
    (1) strongly disagree to (6) strongly agree. Scores on the items are
    summed for AOT and for NFC separately (after reverse scoring
    items that are formulated negatively). Higher scores on AOT
    characterize a greater tendency toward open-minded thinking.
    The reliability of the AOT was good: the internal consistency

    (Cronbach’s alpha) was 0.82. Higher scores on the NFC represent a
    greater tendency to engage in and enjoy effortful cognitive en-
    deavors. Reliability of the NFC was also good: the internal con-
    sistency (Cronbach’s alpha) was 0.80.

    2.2.2. Critical thinking skills tests
    The critical thinking skills tests consisted of sixteen tasks (see

    Appendix A for an example task in each category). The tasks in the
    pre-test, immediate, and delayed post-test were structurally
    equivalent, but surface features (cover stories) differed.

    The reasoning tasks examined the heuristic tendency: 1) to be
    influenced by intense personal and case evidence in favor of more
    representative statistical evidence (two causal base-rate tasks
    adapted from Fong et al., 1986), 2) to base judgments on prior
    belief and intuition without taking sufficient account of the base-
    rate frequency (two non-causal base-rate tasks adapted from De
    Neys & Glumicic, 2008), 3) to neglect simple and fundamental
    qualitative rules of probability in conjunction problems in which a
    conjunction cannot be more probable than one of its ingredients
    (two conjunction tasks, adapted from Tversky & Kahneman, 1983),
    4) to shift outcomes when the same information is framed in
    different ways as in cases of gains and risky options (two framing
    tasks adapted from Tversky & Kahneman, 1981), 5) to evaluate the
    information given in a 2 � 2 contingency table unequally, that is,
    to base estimations on already experienced evidence and disre-
    gard some of the presented evidence (two covariation tasks
    adapted from Wasserman, Dorner, & Kao, 1990), 6) to examine the
    tendency to verify rules rather than to falsify them (two Wason
    selection tasks adapted from Stanovich, 2009, and Wason &
    Shapiro, 1971), and 7) to examine the tendency to evaluate the
    logical validity of arguments on the basis of one’s prior beliefs
    about the truth of conclusions on syllogistic reasoning tasks
    (Evans, 2003; Markovits & Nantel, 1989; Sa et al., 1999). In syllo-
    gistic reasoning tasks a conclusion is drawn from two given pre-
    mises or assumed propositions and participants have to indicate
    whether or not the conclusion follows logically from the premises.
    Different types of inconsistent (i.e., the validity of the conclusion is
    in conflict with its believability) syllogistic tasks were included
    (one of each type): affirming the antecedent or modus ponens (if p
    then q, p therefore q; valid), affirmation of the consequent (if p
    then q, q therefore p; invalid), denial of the antecedent (if p then q,
    not p therefore not q; invalid) and denial of the consequent, or
    modus tollens (if p then q, not q therefore not p; valid).

    The content of the surface features (cover stories) of the tasks
    was adapted to the interests of students in the economics domain.
    The format of the tasks differed; a multiple-choice format with two
    to five answer options (depending on task characteristics) was used
    (the correct answer based upon rational reasoning strategies and
    incorrect answers related to biased reasoning). The internal con-
    sistency (Cronbach’s alpha) on the three tests was 0.50, 0.70 and
    0.73, respectively. Modest reliability for the pre-test composite
    score could be expected and is in line with previous research (de
    Bruin, Parker, & Fischhoff, 2007; West et al., 2008). It should be
    noted that reliability on the post-tests was much higher. It is not
    uncommon for pre-tests to show a lower reliability as performance
    prior to instruction is often more random or variable than perfor-
    mance on post-tests after instruction.

    2.2.3. Critical thinking instructions
    The critical thinking instructions (see Table 1 for an overview of

    the study design) in the experimental conditions consisted of
    computer-based presentation (visual and auditory information) of
    15 min. in which the features of critical thinking, its importance, the
    required reasoning skills, the dispositions, and the risk of biased
    thinking and fallacies in thinking were explained. Examples and

    Table 1
    Overview of study design.

    Experimental conditions Video-based critical thinking
    instruction

    Practice 4 out of 7
    categories of tasks

    Additional prompts
    on practiced tasks

    Control No (unrelated video) Yes No
    Critical thinking instruction Yes Yes No
    Critical thinking þ self-explanation prompts Yes Yes Yes
    Critical thinking þ activation prompts Yes Yes Yes

    A. Heijltjes et al. / Learning and Instruction 29 (2014) 31e42 35

    demonstrations of all task categories were provided, referring back
    to the tasks seen in the pre-test, which could have allowed par-
    ticipants to mentally correct initially erroneous responses. As an
    example, the base-rate fallacy was demonstrated by a slide pre-
    senting an image of a person, not to identify as male or female, with
    a guitar, beer, and some engineering tools. Next to the image the
    visual text appears: base-rate fallacy, group: 990 women, 10 men.
    At the same time the following auditory information was given:
    “The base-rate fallacy is a thinking failure that occurs when the sta-
    tistical distribution of a population is ignored. For example when we
    select a participant randomly out of 1000 participants with 990
    women and 10 men, and we will tell you that this person is called Sam,
    loves drinking beer and listening to hardrock music, and has graduated
    as mechanical engineer, then most people tend to find it most likely
    that this person is a man. In that case they ignore that the total group
    contains of 10 men only” (NB: a different name was used in the
    video, a Dutch name that can also be given to both men and
    women, but for the sake of clarity we used an English name here).
    The computer controlled the sequence and pace of the video, but
    participants could pause, forward and rewind, although observa-
    tions during the experiment suggested they hardly made use of
    these options The control group received a 15 min. digital video on
    an unrelated topic (i.e., what happens in your brain when you are in
    love).

    2.2.4. Practice phase
    After the video-instruction all participants were exposed to a

    business-case from an economics course containing a description
    of a coffee manufacturer who had to decide about marketing,
    quality control, extending the assortment, and the sustainability
    measures. Four categories of tasks (i.e., a contingency task, a
    conjunction task, a non-causal base-rate task, and a Wason selec-
    tion task) were practiced with a similar format as the tasks used in
    the tests, but the cover stories of the practice tasks were derived
    from the business case. Participants did not receive feedback on the
    quality of their performance on the practice tasks.

    Participants in the control condition and the critical thinking
    instruction only condition performed the practice tasks without
    additional prompts. In the instruction plus self-explanation con-
    dition, prompts were given after each task to self-explain how the
    answer was obtained: ‘Explain by using keywords how you’ve
    come to the answer’. In the instruction plus activation prompts
    condition, prompts were given prior to the task to focus on the
    relevant factors. Participants were prompted with hints: ‘search for
    confirmation and refutation’ on the contingency task, ‘think of the
    logical probability’ on the conjunction task, ‘think of the statistical
    distribution’ on the non-causal base-rate task, and ‘violation?’ on
    the Wason selection task.

    To measure whether mental effort invested in practicing tasks
    differed between conditions, a 9-point subjective rating scale
    ranging from (1) very, very low effort to (9) very, very high effort
    (Paas, 1992) was applied after each task in the practice phase.
    Mental effort is an indicator of actual cognitive load, and this scale

    is widely used in educational research (for a review see Van Gog &
    Paas, 2008).

    2.2.5. Procedure
    The experiment was run in 11 sessions in a computer room at

    the university with 15e22 participants per session and all condi-
    tions represented in each session; participants had been randomly
    assigned to one of the conditions prior to the experiment. All of the
    materials were delivered in a computer-based environment that
    was created for this experiment and participants could work at
    their own pace. They first completed the pre-test and dispositions
    tests. Then, participants in the experimental conditions received
    the critical thinking instruction, while participants in the control
    condition watched the video on an unrelated topic. Subsequently,
    all participants read the business case and performed the practice
    tasks, with or without prompts depending on their assigned con-
    dition. Finally, they completed the immediate post-test. Three
    weeks later, all participants were requested via e-mail to complete
    the delayed post-test online.

    2.2.6. Data analysis
    For each correct answer on the critical thinking skills pre-test,

    immediate post-test, and delayed post-test, 1 point was assigned,
    resulting in a maximum score of

    8

    points for practiced tasks, and

    8

    points for not-practiced tasks, on each test.

    Ratings on the AOT and the NFC were summed after reverse
    scoring negatively formulated items, resulting in a maximum
    score on the AOT of 246 and on the NFC of 108. Partial Eta-squared
    (hp

    2) values were computed to estimate the magnitude of group
    differences prior to treatment (i.e., pretest) in test performance
    and critical thinking dispositions, and Cramer’s V was computed
    for group differences with regard to educational background.
    Further, Odds Ratios (ORs) were computed to estimate the
    magnitude of dichotomous variables that might predict dropout
    from immediate to delayed post-test, and Cohen’s d-values were
    computed to estimate the standardized difference in average age,
    AOT, NFC, pre-test performance, immediate post-test performance,
    and invested mental effort during instruction between dropouts
    and non-dropouts. R2 values were computed to estimate age as a
    predictor of post-test scores and AOT and NFC as predictors of pre-
    test scores. Finally, standardized regression coefficients (betas)
    were computed for predictor variables in the multilevel model for
    immediate and delayed post-test performance. To take the hier-
    archical structure of the data into account, not the raw test per-
    formance standard deviation (as linked to the standardized beta
    formula calculated in single-level multiple regression analysis) but
    the residual (adjusted) standard deviation of test performance was
    used to calculate the betas. The lowest level variance is then “the
    amount of variation in the outcome measure attributable to the
    individual observation after appropriate controls have been made”
    (Schagen & Elliot, 2004, p. 13). Although effect sizes tend to be
    slightly larger in the residual standard deviation approach as
    compared to a raw standard deviation approach, “such

    A. Heijltjes et al. / Learning and Instruction 29 (2014) 31e4236

    calculations are considered appropriate because they explicitly
    model the extent and impact of clustering in the data” (Schagen &
    Elliot, 2004, p. 13).

    5.00

    6.00

    7.00

    8

    3. Results

    For all analyzes, a significance level of 0.05 was used, except for
    post-hoc comparisons between conditions for which a significance
    level of 0.05/6 was used (note that the uncorrected p-values are
    reported). Random assignment of participants to conditions had
    been successful; there were no significant differences between
    conditions in critical thinking pre-test performance on practiced
    tasks, F(3, 179) ¼ 1.36, p ¼ 0.258, hp2 ¼ 0.02, not-practiced tasks, F(3,
    179) ¼ 0.44, p ¼ 0.723, hp2 ¼ 0.01, critical thinking dispositions (AOT:
    F(3, 179) ¼ 0.33, p ¼ 0.803, hp2 ¼ 0.01; NFC: F(3, 179) ¼ 1.2, p ¼ 0.311,
    hp
    2 ¼ 0 02), and educational background, c2(6) ¼ 8.38, p ¼ 0.212,

    V ¼ 0.15. Regarding the following analyzes, it is worth noting that
    the response rate on the delayed post-test was approximately 45%
    (N ¼ 85): control (n ¼ 16), critical thinking instruction (n ¼ 25),
    critical thinking instruction plus self-explanation prompts (n ¼ 21),
    and critical thinking instruction plus activation prompts (n ¼ 23).
    Logistic regression to investigate whether participants who
    completed the immediate post-test only (i.e., dropouts) differed
    from participants who completed both the immediate and the
    delayed post-test (i.e., persisters) reveal that dropouts could not be
    predicted based on AOT scores (p ¼ 0.284, dropouts M ¼ 179.88,
    SD ¼ 17.09, persisters M ¼ 177.36, SD 14.45, d ¼ 0.16), NFC scores
    (p ¼ 0.432, dropouts M ¼ 77.98, SD ¼ 9.11, persisters M ¼ 79.05, SD
    9.44, d ¼ �0.12), educational background (p ¼ 0.851, OR ¼ 1.04),
    gender (p ¼ 0.188, OR ¼ 0.65), pre-test performance (p ¼ 0.513,
    dropouts M ¼ 7.68, SD ¼ 2.31, persisters M ¼ 7.92, SD ¼ 2.55,
    d ¼ �0.01), immediate-post-test performance (p ¼ 0.283, dropouts
    M ¼ 9.11, SD ¼ 3.31, persisters M ¼ 9.60, SD ¼ 2.76, d ¼ �0.16), in-
    struction conditions (p ¼ 0.862, OR ¼ 0.95) or invested mental
    effort during instructions (p ¼ 0.342, dropouts M ¼ 3.65, SD ¼ 1.30,
    persisters M ¼ 3.83, SD ¼ 1.20, d ¼ �0.14). Only age was a significant
    predictor of dropout (p ¼ 0.004, dropouts M ¼ 30.6, SD ¼ 7.35,
    persisters M ¼ 27.9, SD ¼ 5.12, d ¼ 0.43), indicating a small to me-
    dium effect (Cohen, 1992); however this effect seems of minor
    importance as age was not a predictor of immediate post-test
    scores (p ¼ 0.089, R2 ¼ 0.02) or delayed post-test scores
    (p ¼ 0.076, R2 ¼ 0.04). All in all, the p-values in combination with
    the measures of effect size indicate that the groups who did and did
    not complete the delayed posttest were comparable on the re-
    ported variables prior to treatment.

    To explore whether invested mental effort during practice
    differed among instruction conditions an ANOVA was conducted,
    which showed no significant differences among conditions, F(3,
    179) ¼ 1.30, p ¼ 0.276, hp2 ¼ 0.02.

    Note. Group A: control, Group B: critical thinking instruction, group C: critical thinking instruction + self-
    explanation prompts, Group D: critical thinking + activation prompts. Covariates in the model kept constant on
    their mean value: Scores on pre-test reasoning tasks, invested mental effort during instruction, Actively Open-
    minded Thinking, and Need for Cognition.

    0.00

    1.00

    2.00

    3.00

    4.00

    Immediate Post-test Delayed Post-test

    S
    co

    re
    s

    ra
    ng

    e
    0

    Group A Group B Group C Group D

    Fig. 1. Estimated marginal means of practiced tasks on immediate and delayed post-
    test by instruction conditions.

    3.1. Dispositions and pre-test performance

    To test the hypothesis that participants with higher scores on
    dispositions (i.e., AOT and NFC) would score better on the pre-test
    of critical thinking skills than participants with lower scores on
    these dispositions (Hypothesis 1), a multiple regression analysis
    was performed. On the dispositions tests, data from 11 participants
    were lost due to a technical error. Their scores were replaced with
    the average sample scores. In line with our hypothesis, the dispo-
    sitions AOT and NFC significantly predicted pre-test critical
    thinking skills, F(2, 180) ¼ 11.55, p < 0.001, R2 ¼ 0.11, indicating a medium effect (Cohen, 1992). Regarding the standardized co- efficients the AOT scores showed a slightly higher impact, b ¼ 0.22, t(180) ¼ 2.95, p ¼ 0.004, than the NFC scores, b ¼ 0.20, t(180) ¼

    2.64, p ¼ 0.009, indicating a small to medium effect (Lipsey &
    Wilson, 2001).

    3.2. Effects of instructions and dispositions on immediate and
    delayed post-test performance

    To include not only participants who completed the immediate
    and the delayed test but to use as much information as possible, a
    multilevel regression model was used. The following fixed factors
    were included in the model consistently: pre-test scores (as co-
    variate), dispositions scores (i.e., AOT and NFC), invested mental
    effort scores during instruction, instruction conditions, practice
    (i.e., practiced tasks versus not practiced tasks), and test moment
    (i.e., immediate post-test scores and delayed post-test scores). A
    student-level random intercept and random slope for practice
    were included in the model. No abnormal departures from
    normality or outliers were found. Figs. 1 and 2 display the mean
    reasoning scores of practiced and not-practiced tasks in each
    condition on the immediate and delayed post-test. Table 5 pre-
    sents the adjusted means along with their standard errors and 95%
    confidence intervals for every combination of condition by prac-
    tice by test moment, keeping all quantitative covariates constant
    at their mean value.

    Regarding the explorative question of whether students with
    higher scores on dispositions would also benefit more from the
    critical instruction than students with lower scores on disposi-
    tions, or whether instruction would be equally effective for all
    participants (Question 1), Table 2 shows that the dispositions and
    critical thinking performance were correlated significantly even
    after instruction (i.e., mainly between AOT and post-test scores),
    however the multilevel regression analysis, using Satterthwaite
    approximation for the degrees of freedom in SPSS version 21
    (Table 3), revealed that no interactions were found between scores
    on dispositions (i.e., AOT and NFC) and instructions, between
    dispositions and practice, or between dispositions and test
    moment.

    The hypotheses that instruction would have an effect on post-
    test performance (Hypothesis 2a) when combined with practice
    (Hypothesis 2c), that prompts during practice would further
    enhance the effects of instructions and practice (Hypothesis 3a),
    and that these effects remained the same after a three-week delay
    (Hypothesis 2b and 3b), were also tested by means of the

    Table 3
    Fixed effects for predictors of reasoning scores.

    Parameter df F p

    Intercept 1186.06 43.63 <0.001 Conditions 3289.58 3.94 0.009 Practice 1302.88 3.17 0.076 Test moment 1255.56 51.95 <0.001 Conditions � practice 3302.20 10.85 <0.001 Conditions � test moment 3254.98 1.10 0.351 Conditions � practice � test moment 3259.66 0.28 0.842 Practice � test moment 1260.12 60.93 <0.001 Pre-test reasoning scores 1166.67 82.39 <0.001 Invested mental effort 1173.95 4.62 0.033 NFC 1262.90 0.11 0.747 AOT 1267.42 1.96 0.163 NFC � test moment 1255.12 1.04 0.309 AOT � test moment 1270.45 0.85 0.358 NFC � conditions 3166.92 0.96 0.414 AOT � conditions 3178.35 1.95 0.124 NFC � practice 1217.96 1.50 0.223 AOT � practice 1235.33 2.55 0.112

    Note. Intercept based on not-practiced tasks on the immediate post-test; Practice:
    practiced tasks (code ‘1’) versus not-practiced tasks (code ‘0’); Test moment: im-
    mediate post-test (code ‘0’) versus delayed post-test (code ‘1’). AOT: Actively Open-
    minded Thinking, NFC: Need for Cognition, Conditions: control, critical thinking
    instruction, critical thinking instruction þ self-explanation prompts, critical thin-
    king þ activation prompts.

    Note. Group A: control, Group B: critical thinking instruction, group C: critical thinking instruction + self-
    explanation prompts, Group D: critical thinking + activation prompts. Covariates in the model kept constant on
    their mean value: Scores on pre-test reasoning tasks, invested mental effort during instruction, Actively Open-
    minded Thinking and Need for Cognition.

    0.00
    1.00
    2.00
    3.00
    4.00
    5.00
    6.00
    Immediate Post-test Delayed Post-test
    S
    co
    re
    s
    ra
    ng

    e
    0-

    8
    Group A Group B Group C Group D

    Fig. 2. Estimated marginal means of not-practiced tasks on immediate and delayed
    post-test by instruction conditions.

    A. Heijltjes et al. / Learning and Instruction 29 (2014) 31e42 37

    multilevel regression analysis (Table 3). This analysis showed
    significant main effects of instruction condition and test moment,
    and significant interaction effects between instruction condition
    and practice, and between test moment and practice. Table 4
    shows the B-values for the main and interaction effects and the
    effect sizes (betas). Note that the intercept is based on not-
    practiced tasks of the immediate post-test in the control condi-
    tion (i.e., group A). The betas for pre-test reasoning scores and
    invested mental effort indicate that pre-test reasoning score has a
    strong positive effect on immediate and delayed post-test per-
    formance, while invested mental effort during instruction appears
    to have a small to moderate negative effect on performance. In
    line with the significance tests presented in Table 3, the signifi-
    cance tests and betas in Table 4 appear to indicate that NFC and
    AOT hardly influence post-test performance. Further, the condi-
    tion by practice interaction appears to be a strong effect; in the
    control condition, immediate post-test performance is consider-
    ably lower on practiced tasks (notice the negative beta), while in
    the other three conditions immediate post-test performance is
    much higher on the practiced tasks than on the not practiced
    tasks.

    To investigate the interaction effect between condition and
    practice further, simple effect analyzes taking into account the
    factors practice (i.e., practiced versus not-practiced tasks) and test
    moment (i.e., immediate and delayed post-test), revealed that
    participants in the instruction conditions outperformed the con-
    trols on practiced tasks on the immediate post-test (all p < 0.001) and on the delayed post-test (all p smaller than 0.002). LSD post-

    Table 2
    Correlations between critical thinking performance and dispositions (AOT and NFC) on pre

    1 2

    1. AOT e
    2. NFC 0.321*** e
    3. Pre-test practiced tasks 0.220** 0.173*
    4. Pre-test not-practiced tasks 0.244** 0.263***
    5. Immediate post-test practiced-tasks 0.323*** 0.192**
    6. Immediate post-test not-practiced tasks 0.259*** 0.114
    7. Delayed post-test practiced tasks 0.330** 0.123
    8. Delayed post-test not-practiced tasks 0.222* 0.106

    Note. AOT: actively open-minded thinking scores, NFC: need for cognition scores, *** Signi

    hoc tests on practiced tasks on the immediate and delayed
    post-test revealed no significant differences between instruction
    conditions (immediate test: all p larger than 0.484, delayed test: all
    p larger than 0.053). On not-practiced tasks, only participants in the
    instruction condition with self-explanation prompts outperformed
    those in the control condition on the immediate post-test
    (p < 0.001) but not on the delayed post-test (p ¼ 0.199). LSD post- hoc tests showed that other comparisons between conditions on not-practiced tasks were not significant on either the immediate post-test (all p larger than 0.020) or the delayed post-test (all p larger than 0.030). Note that the uncorrected p-values are reported, but these are no longer significant after correction for multiple tests (i.e., 0.05/6).

    Finally, the practice by test moment interaction (Table 4) in-
    dicates that there is a decrease in performance on the practiced
    tasks from immediate to delayed post-test whereas there is no
    change in performance on not-practiced tasks; the beta of
    practice by test moment suggests that this is a strong effect. Note
    though (Table 3), that there was no significant interaction with
    condition.

    4. Discussion

    The aim of this study was to examine a) the impact of individual
    differences in dispositions and b) effects of instructions, practice,

    -test, immediate post-test and delayed post-test of practiced and not-practiced tasks.

    3 4 5 6 7

    e

    0.364*** e
    0.379*** 0.317*** e
    0.341*** 0.502*** 0.440*** e
    0.240* 0.198 0.653*** 0.434*** e
    0.283** 0.430*** 0.355** 0.655*** 0.428***

    ficant at the 0.001 level, ** Significant at the 0.001 level, * Significant at the 0.05 level.

    Table 4
    Fixed effects estimates and covariance estimates of reasoning scores.

    Parameter B (SE) df t p Effect size (beta)

    Level 1 Fixed effects
    Intercept 1.98 (0.43) 208.38 4.62 <0.001 Group B 0.52 (0.29) 289.85 1.81 0.072 0.48 Group C 0.99 (0.29) 289.48 3.44 <0.001 0.91 Group D 0.55 (0.28) 288.78 1.94 0.054 0.51 Practice �1.09 (0.29) 302.01 �3.74 <0.001 �1.01 Test moment 0.92 (0.35) 262.03 2.66 0.008 0.85 Practice � test moment �1.49 (0.48) 267.24 �3.11 0.002 �1.38 Group B � practice 1.86 (0.40) 302.10 4.66 <0.001 1.72 Group C � practice 1.41 (0.40) 301.15 3.53 <0.001 1.30 Group D � practice 2.07 (0.39) 302.64 5.31 <0.001 1.91 Group B � test moment 0.63 (0.45) 256.05 1.40 0.161 0.58 Group C � test moment �0.02 (0.46) 259.51 �0.05 0.962 �0.02 Group D � test moment �0.11 (0.45) 258.31 �0.24 0.807 0.10 Group B � practice � test moment 0.01 (0.62) 259.53 0.02 0.983 0.01 Group C x Practice � Test moment �0.28 (0.64) 264.55 �0.44 0.664 �0.26 Group D � practice � test moment �0.44 (0.63) 264.58 �0.70 0.487 �0.40 Pre-test reasoning scores 0.32 (0.04) 166.67 9.08 <0.001 0.71 Invested mental effort �0.14 (0.07) 173.95 �2.15 0.033 �0.16 NFC 0.01 (0.02) 198.80 0.52 0.602 0.09 AOT �0.01 (0.01) 213.56 �1.18 0.241 �0.18 Group B � AOT 0.03 (0.02) 169.30 1.78 0.076 0.40 Group C � AOT 0.04 (0.01) 186.69 2.07 0.040 0.45 Group D � AOT 0.03 (0.01) 177.66 1.91 0.058 0.40 Group B � NFC �0.02 (0.03) 163.57 �0.60 0.552 �0.14 Group C � NFC �0.04 (0.03) 175.93 �1.46 0.146 �0.33 Group D � NFC �0.00 (0.03) 167.16 �0.09 0.932 �0.02 AOT � practice 0.01 (0.01) 235.33 1.60 0.112 0.19 NFC � practice 0.02 (0.01) 217.96 1.22 0.223 0.15 AOT � test moment 0.01 (0.01) 270.45 0.92 0.358 0.11 NFC � test moment �0.01 (0.01) 255.12 �1.02 0.309 �0.11 Level 2 Random parameters

    cov (SE) Wald Z p
    Intercepts participants 0.53 (0.13) 4.25 <0.001 Practice participants 1.05 (0.26) 4.02 <0.001 Residuals 1.17 (0.11) 10.21 <0.001

    Note. Intercept based on not-practiced tasks on the immediate post-test; Practice: practiced tasks (code ‘1’) versus not-practiced tasks (code ‘0’); Test moment: immediate
    post-test (code ‘0’) versus delayed post-test (code ‘1’). Conditions: group A: control condition, group B: CT instruction, group C: CT instruction þ self-explanation prompts,
    group D: CT-Instruction þ activation prompts; NFC: Need for Cognition; AOT: Actively Open-minded Thinking.
    Effect sizes: 0.1 small; 0.25 medium, 0.40 large (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001).

    A. Heijltjes et al. / Learning and Instruction 29 (2014) 31e4238

    and prompts on economics students’ critical thinking as measured
    by their performance on reasoning tasks on an immediate and
    delayed post-test.

    In line with our first hypothesis (Hypothesis 1), the results
    clearly indicate that those participants with higher scores on dis-
    positions (i.e., AOT and NFC) scored significantly better on the

    Table 5
    Estimated marginal means, standard errors and 95% confidence intervals of prac-
    ticed and not-practiced task categories on the immediate and delayed post-test by
    groups.

    Group Test-moment

    Immediate post-test Delayed post-test

    M SE CI M SE CI

    Practiced tasks A 4.83 0.34 [4.17, 5.50] 3.98 0.23 [3.53, 4.43]
    B 5.95 0.27 [5.42, 6.49] 4.64 0.21 [4.04, 4.88]
    C 5.87 0.29 [5.29, 6.44] 4.94 0.21 [4.52, 5.36]
    D 5.60 0.28 [5.05, 6.16] 4.50 0.21 [4.09, 4.90]

    Not-practiced
    tasks

    A 2.29 0.34 [1.63, 2.96] 2.88 0.23 [2.43, 3.33]
    B 5.31 0.27 [4.77, 5.84] 5.23 0.21 [4.81, 5.65]
    C 4.40 0.29 [3.82, 4.98] 5.26 0.21 [4.83, 5.68]
    D 4.55 0.28 [3.40, 5.11] 5.47 0.21 [5.07, 5.87]

    Note. Group A: control, Group B: instruction only, Group C: instruction and self-
    explanation prompts, Group D: instruction and activation prompts. Covariates in
    the model kept constant on their mean value: Scores on pre-test reasoning tasks,
    invested mental effort during instruction, AOT: Actively Open-minded, Thinking
    NFC: Need for Cognition.

    initial assessment of critical thinking skills than participants with
    lower scores on dispositions. These results converge with findings
    in other studies on argument evaluation (Stanovich & West, 1997),
    syllogistic reasoning (Macpherson & Stanovich, 2007), and covari-
    ation judgment (Sa et al., 2005; West et al., 2008). Regarding the
    question of whether students with higher scores on dispositions
    would benefit more from the critical thinking instruction than
    students with lower scores on dispositions (Question 1), our results
    showed no significant interaction effects between instruction
    conditions and dispositions (i.e., AOT and NFC). Thus, it seems that
    despite the relationship between dispositions and pre-test perfor-
    mance, all students benefitted equally from instructions in terms of
    post-test performance; students who score higher on dispositions
    did not benefit more from instructions than students who score
    lower on dispositions or vice versa. These results indicate that the
    active search for evidence against one’s own beliefs, plans, or goals,
    and the ability to weigh available evidence fairly (i.e., AOT; Baron,
    2008), and the intrinsic cognitive motivation (i.e., NFC; Cacioppo
    et al., 1996), which are both important features of the reflective
    mind (Stanovich, 2011), appear to regulate reasoning, but not
    learning to reason. These explorative findings suggest that dispo-
    sitions might be overruled by extrinsic factors in the educational
    context; declarative instruction to search for general and under-
    lying principles and the instruction to give considerable thoughts
    to the instructions seems to have canceled out any influence of

    A. Heijltjes et al. / Learning and Instruction 29 (2014) 31e42 39

    AOT and NFC on learning. These results indicate that instructions
    were effective for all participants regardless of their disposition
    scores.

    Our second hypothesis was that critical thinking instruction
    would enhance performance on critical thinking skills compared to
    no instruction, both immediately (Hypothesis 2a) and after a three-
    week delay (Hypothesis 2b), though only when combined with
    practice (Hypothesis 2c). Indeed, it seems that the combination of
    instructions with practice is crucial and has a large effect: on both
    the immediate and the delayed post-test the improvements on
    reasoning skills compared to the control condition were found on
    task categories that were practiced with the business case, but not
    on task categories that had not been practiced. There was one
    exception: on the immediate test, the self-explanation condition
    performed better than the control condition on not-practiced tasks.
    We will return to this finding below, when discussing effects of
    prompting (Hypothesis 3). Our results are in line with and extend
    findings from previous studies in which it was found that explicit
    instructions improved critical thinking (e.g., Abrami et al., 2008;
    Angeli & Valanides, 2009), that rational thinking to override Type
    1 processes is trainable through explicit instruction (e.g., Nisbett
    et al., 1987) and that even short instructions can have a persistent
    impact on becoming more rational at reasoning (Larrick et al.,
    1990). On the other hand, our results also show that critical
    thinking instruction without practice was not sufficient to lead to
    sustained effects and that practice had an added value for reasoning
    instruction. Practice seems to play a key role and presumably leads
    to better learning by allowing participants to integrate and elabo-
    rate information from the instructions (e.g., Bransford et al., 1986)
    with tasks in the economics context. Interestingly, mental effort
    during practice did not differ between conditions, and combined
    with the differences in post-test scores on practiced tasks, this
    suggests that the processes engaged in during practice differ as a
    function of instructions, but that the cognitive demands imposed
    by those processes do not differ (i.e., practice becomes more
    efficient).

    The results only lend partial support to our third hypothesis
    that after instruction, combining practice with prompts, would
    foster acquisition of critical thinking skills compared to instruction
    and practice only both immediately (Hypothesis 3a) and after a
    three-week delay (Hypothesis 3b). Activation prompts, which we
    expected to be able to affect critical thinking performance by
    redirecting attention to relevant cues, did not lead to better test
    performance than instructions only. This finding might indicate
    that instructions were sufficient to redirect attention and inhibit
    initial automatic responses during practice. Self-explanation
    prompts, which we expected to foster the proper use of avail-
    able knowledge (Roy & Chi, 2005) also did not foster performance
    on practiced reasoning tasks compared to instructions only.
    Interestingly, however, participants in this condition performed
    better on not-practiced reasoning tasks on the immediate post-
    test than the control condition, suggesting that prompting self-
    explanations established a kind of transfer from practiced to
    not-practiced tasks, at least in the short run (this effect was no
    longer present on the delayed test). This result converges with
    studies on the benefits of self-explanation of problem solving
    tasks that showed that self-explanation can foster transfer (e.g.,
    Aleven & Koedinger, 2002; Lombrozo, 2006; Renkl, 2005; Rittle-
    Johnson, 2006). However, given that the difference with the
    other instruction conditions was not significant, and that the effect
    was no longer present on the delayed test, this effect should be
    interpreted with caution and should be replicated in future
    research. It might also be worthwhile to attempt to deepen
    instructional effects of self-explanation in future studies by

    explicitly teaching it as a meta-strategy or by providing feedback
    on self-explanations, as this might further enhance the effects of
    self-explanation on critical thinking and potentially on transfer to
    not-practiced tasks.

    In sum, based on these findings, we can conclude that dis-
    positions only have an impact on pre-test reasoning perfor-
    mance but not on learning of reasoning skills. Secondly we can
    conclude that explicit reasoning instructions should be com-
    bined with opportunities for practice to have an effect on crit-
    ical thinking performance. Thirdly, prompting self-explanation
    during practice might be an interesting instructional method as
    we found some indications of transfer in terms of performance
    benefits on not-practiced reasoning tasks compared to the
    control condition; however, further research would be necessary
    to establish the merits of this method for teaching reasoning
    skills.

    This study has some limitations. First, for practical reasons it
    was not possible to administer the delayed post-test at the
    institute as the rest of the experiment, however the web-based
    environment that was used for the delayed post-test was the
    same as the one used during the session at the institute. Sec-
    ondly, despite the fact that students could complete the delayed
    test wherever they wanted, there was a rather high attrition rate
    on the delayed test. However, it should be noted that participants
    who did complete the delayed test did not differ from partici-
    pants who did not on pre-test performance, dispositions,
    educational background, gender, invested mental effort during
    instruction, and performance on the immediate test. Third, we
    did not include conditions that received the prompts during
    practice but no critical thinking instruction. The reason for that
    was that it can be questioned whether prompts would have a
    beneficial effect for students lacking knowledge about the tasks
    (i.e., without instructions first). As Stanovich and Stanovich
    (2010) stated, on the tasks we used, suppressing the initial
    response (Type 1 reasoning) is only helpful when a better
    response is available to substitute for it. And regarding self-
    explanations it has been shown that without any knowledge, it
    would be unlikely that students would be able to provide deep,
    principled explanations, which have been shown to be the most
    effective (Renkl, 1997). Under conditions of very low prior
    knowledge, instructional explanations have been shown to be
    most effective (Renkl, 2002). The instructions provided students
    with a basis for responding to the prompts, which they would
    not have had otherwise. Given the low effectiveness of prompt-
    ing even after instructions, it seems highly unlikely that
    prompting without instructions first would have had an effect.
    Nevertheless, we did not directly test this, and future research
    might resolve the question of whether self-explanation or acti-
    vation prompts have an impact on reasoning improvements
    without prior critical thinking instruction. Finally, it should be
    kept in mind that this study defined critical thinking in terms of
    reasoning skills, and that the findings therefore may not gener-
    alize to other definitions of critical thinking.

    Despite these limitations, this study provided promising
    results for (economics) educators who wish to enhance their
    students’ critical thinking skills, by showing that such skills
    can be enhanced through relatively simple explicit in-
    structions eprovided they are combined with practice. The
    findings are especially promising given that this experimental
    study was of relatively short duration; teaching critical
    thinking throughout the economics curriculum might help
    students to learn to avoid biased reasoning and better prepare
    them for decision-making in dynamic and complex business
    environments.

    Appendix A. Example of each category of tasks used on critical thinking tests (* [ right option; 1 [ category practiced;
    2 [ category not practiced).

    Conjunction task
    (1)

    A kitchen manufacturer wants to reposition itself in the kitchen industry. The goal is to
    increase the market share to 10%. Based on a strength/weakness analysis, measures have
    been taken to improve market share, pay more attention to the entrepreneurship of the
    employees, and conduct a more value-oriented communication campaign towards
    customers.
    Which option is most likely?
    Option 1. The market share of the kitchen manufacturer will increase by 3%.*
    Option 2: The market share of the kitchen manufacturer will increase by 5%.
    Option 3: The market share of the kitchen manufacturer will increase by 3%, and the
    satisfaction of customers and employees will improve.
    Option 4: The market share of the kitchen manufacturer will increase by 5%, and the
    satisfaction of customers and employees will improve.

    Explanation: Options 2, 3 and 4 violate the conjunction rule as a conjunction cannot be
    more probable than one of its constituents.

    Contingency task
    (1)

    An insurance company claimed that too much time was spent on advising customers
    without any results. Costs and benefits were not balanced. Applying a new advising
    system should improve this situation. The new system was used on a part of the
    customers. A student was assigned to evaluate the effect of the new system to determine
    the systems’ time efficiency. The student obtained the information below and concludes
    that the application of the new system resulted in saving of time.

    New system used New system not used
    Saving time 325 260
    No time saving 90 55

    Table: number of customers on which the system was applied or not and time was saved
    or not.
    Do you agree with the student?
    Option 1. fully agree
    Option 2. agree
    Option 3. neither agree nor disagree
    Option 4. disagree*
    Option 5. fully disagree*

    Explanation: Option 1, 2 and 3 refer to the tendency to evaluate the information given in
    a 2 x 2 contingency table unequally.

    Non-causal

    base-rate task
    (1)

    A renowned regional company has two vacancies for junior economists. This company
    has very good experiences with economics master graduates from a specific University
    for over 15 years. The company management will continue this policy of hiring graduates
    from this University. A new personnel manager, however, suggests attracting economics
    graduates of a Polytechnic University. The manager argued that he gained outstanding
    experiences in a business where he worked formerly with a polytechnic graduate who
    functioned at an excellent level from the outset. The personnel manager believes that
    polytechnic graduates work equally well as master graduates from the specific
    University.
    What should the management of the company best decide?
    Option 1: Definitely chose for master graduates of the specific University.*
    Option 2: Probably chose for master graduates of the specific University.*
    Option 3: Probably chose for graduates of the Polytechnic University.
    Option 4: Definitely chose for graduates of the Polytechnic University.

    Explanation: People who choose option 3 or 4 neglect the base-rate, for example
    motivated by personal and case evidence in favor of more representative statistical
    evidence.

    Causal

    base-rate task
    (2)

    A study had 1000 participants. Among the participants there were 25 men and 975
    women. Sam is a randomly chosen participant in this study. Sam is 23 years old,
    graduated as mechanical engineer and enjoys going out with friends, likes drinking beer
    and is a fan of hard rock music.
    Which option is most probable?
    Option 1: Sam is a man.
    Option 2: Sam is a woman.*

    Explanation: Option 1 demonstrates the tendency to base judgments on prior belief and
    intuition rather than on logical reasoning (i.e., taking into account the prior probability).

    Framing task
    (2)

    Imagine that you face the following pair of concurrent decisions: First examine both
    decisions, then indicate the options you prefer.

    A. A sure gain of 480 euro.
    B. 25% chance to gain 2000 euro and 75% chance to gain nothing.

    C. A sure loss of 1500 euro.
    D. 75% chance to lose 2000 and 25% chance to lose nothing.

    Options: AC, BC*, AD, BD

    Explanation: The paired choices were presented together but the problem was ‘framed’
    as a pair of separate choices. The combination B&C is superior: 25% chance to win 500
    and 75% chance to lose 1500. For example, with A&D there is 25% chance to win 480
    and 75% chance to lose 1520. In case of gains people tend to chose risk aversive (option
    A) and in case of losses to chose risk seeking (option D).

    Wason selection
    task
    (1)

    Each of the tickets below has a destination on one side and an airline on the other side.
    Here is a rule: If ‘Barcelona’ is on one side of the ticket, then ‘Ryanair’ is on the other
    side of the ticket. Your task is to decide which tickets you would need to turn over in
    order to find out whether or not the rule is being violated.

    Destination
    Barcelona

    Destination
    Madrid

    Airline
    Ryanair

    Airline
    Transavia

    A B C D
    Options: AC, AD*, BC, BD

    Explanation: People who chose other options than AD probably fail to apply logical
    principles, verify rules rather than to falsify them, or demonstrate matching bias by
    selecting options explicitly mentioned in the conditional statement.

    Syllogistic
    reasoning tasks
    (2)

    The category syllogistic reasoning tasks exist of 4 types of tasks.
    In the following assignments, you will be given two premises, which you must assume
    are true. A conclusion from the premises then follows. You must decide whether the
    conclusion follows logically from the premises or not.

    Premises:
    All oil companies are quoted companies.
    Shell is a quoted company.
    Conclusion: Shell is an oil company.
    Option 1. Conclusion follows logically from premises.
    Option 2. Conclusion does not follow logically from premises.*

    Explanation:

    Syllogism type: Affirmation of consequent, invalid

    Premises:
    All mammals walk.
    Dolphins are mammals.
    Conclusion: Dolphins walk.
    Option 1. Conclusion follows logically from premises.*
    Option 2. Conclusion does not follow logically from premises.

    Explanation:

    Syllogism type: Affirmation of antecedent/Modus Ponens, valid

    Premises:
    All things that move love water.
    Cats do not love water.
    Conclusion: Cats do not move.
    Option 1. Conclusion follows logically from premises.*
    Option 2. Conclusion does not follow logically from premises.

    Explanation:

    Syllogism Type: Denial of consequent/ Modus Tollens, valid

    Premises:
    All oil countries are rich.
    Belgium is not an oil country.
    Conclusion: Belgium is not rich.
    Option 1. Conclusion follows logically from premises.
    Option 2. Conclusion does not follow logically from premises.*

    Explanation:

    Syllogism type: Denial of antecedent, invalid

    A. Heijltjes et al. / Learning and Instruction 29 (2014) 31e42 41

    References

    Abrami, P. C., Bernard, R. M., Borokhovski, E., Wade, A., Surkes, M. A., Tamim, R., &
    Zhang, D. (2008). Instructional interventions affecting critical thinking skills
    and dispositions: a stage 1 meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 78,
    1102e1134. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0034654308326084.

    Aleven, A. W., & Koedinger, K. R. (2002). An effective meta-cognitive strategy:
    learning by doing and explaining with a computer-based cognitive tutor.
    Cognitive Science, 26, 147e179. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2602_1.

    Angeli, C., & Valanides, N. (2009). Instructional effects on critical thinking: perfor-
    mance on ill-defined issues. Learning and Instruction, 19, 322e334. http://
    dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2008.06.010.

    Austin, Z., Gregory, P. A., & Chiu, S. (2008). Use of reflection-in-action and self-
    assessment to promote critical thinking among pharmacy students. American
    Journal of Pharmaceutical Education, 72, 1e8.

    Bangert-Drowns, R. L., & Bankert, E. (1990). Meta-analysis of explicit instruction for
    critical thinking. Boston, MA: Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
    American Educational Research Association.

    http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0034654308326084

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2602_1

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2008.06.010

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2008.06.010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref4

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref4

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref4

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref4

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref5

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref5

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref5

    A. Heijltjes et al. / Learning and Instruction 29 (2014) 31e4242

    Baron, J. (2008). Actively open-minded thinking. In J. Baron (Ed.), Thinking and
    deciding (pp. 199e232). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Bransford, J., Sherwood, R., Vye, N., & Rieser, J. (1986). Teaching thinking and
    problem-solving. American Psychologist, 41, 1078e1089. http://dx.doi.org/
    10.1037/0003-066X.41.10.1078.

    de Bruin, W., Parker, A. M., & Fischhoff, B. (2007). Individual differences in adult
    decion-making competence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92,
    938e956. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.5.938.

    De Neys, W., & Glumicic, T. (2008). Conflict monitoring in dual process theories of
    thinking. Cognition, 106, 1248e1299. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.
    2007.06.002.

    Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Feinstein, J. A., Blair, G., & Jarvis, W. (1996). Dispositional
    differences in cognitive motivation: the life and times of individuals varying in
    need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 197e253. http://dx.doi.org/
    10.1037/0033-2909.119.2.197.

    Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., & Kao, C. F. (1984). The efficient assessment of need for
    cognition. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 306e307. http://dx.doi.org/
    10.1207/s15327752jpa4803_13.

    Cohen, J. (1992). Quantitative methods in psychology; a power primer. Psychological
    Bulletin, 112, 155e159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155.

    Evans, J. St. B. T. (2003). In two minds: dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in
    Cognitive Sciences, 7, 454e459. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.08.012.

    Evans, J. St. B. T. (2008). Dual processing account of reasoning, judgment and social
    cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255e278. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/
    annurev.psych.59.103006.093629.

    Evans, J. St. B. T. (2011). Dual-process theories of reasoning: contemporary issues
    and developmental applications. Developmental Review, 31, 86e102. http://
    dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2011.07.007.

    Facione, P. A. (1990). Critical thinking: a statement of expert consensus for purposes of
    educational assessment and instruction. Millbrae, CA: The California Academic
    Press.

    Facione, P. A. (2009). Critical thinking: what it is and why it counts. Retrieved from
    http://www.insightassessment.com/9articles%20WW.html.

    Fong, G. T., Krantz, D. H., & Nisbett, R. E. (1986). The effect of statistical training on
    thinking about everyday problems. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 253e292. http://
    dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(86)90001-0.

    Halpern, D. F. (1998). Teaching critical thinking for transfer across domains: dis-
    positions, skills, structure training, and metacognitive monitoring. American
    Psychologist, 53, 449e455. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.53.4.449.

    Halpern, D. F. (1999). Teaching for critical thinking: helping college students
    develop the skills and dispositions of a critical thinker. New Directions for
    Teaching and Learning, 80, 69e74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/tl.8005.

    Helsdingen, A., Van Gog, T., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2011). Effects of practice
    schedule and critical thinking instruction on learning and transfer of a complex
    judgment task. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 383e398. http://
    dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0022370.

    Jones, A. (2007). Multiplicities or manna from heaven? Critical thinking and the
    disciplinary context. Australian Journal of Education, 51, 84e103.

    Klebba, J. M., & Hamilton, J. G. (2007). Structured case analysis: developing critical
    thinking skills in a marketing case course. Journal of Marketing Education, 29,
    132e139. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0273475307302015.

    Larrick, R. P., Morgan, J. N., & Nisbett, R. E. (1990). Teaching the use of cost-benefits
    reasoning in everyday life. Psychological Science, 1, 362e370. http://dx.doi.org/
    10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00243.x.

    Lehman, D. R., Lempert, R. O., & Nisbett, R. E. (1988). The effects of graduate training
    on reasoning. American Psychologist, 43, 431e442. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
    0003-66X.43.6.431.

    Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. London: Sage.
    Lombrozo, T. (2006). The structure and function of explanations. Trends in Cognitive

    Sciences, 10, 464e470. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.08.004.
    Macpherson, R., & Stanovich, K. E. (2007). Cogitive ability, thinking dispositions, and

    instructional set as predictors of critical thinking. Learning and Individual Dif-
    ferences, 17, 115e127. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2007.05.003.

    Markovits, H., & Nantel, G. (1989). The belief-bias effect in the production and
    evaluation of logical conclusions. Memory & Cognition, 17, 11e17. http://
    dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03199552.

    Matthews, P., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2009). In pursuit of knowledge: comparing self-
    explanations concepts and procedures as pedagogical tools. Journal of Experi-
    mental Child Psychology, 102, 1e21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.08.004.

    McMillan, J. H. (1987). Enhancing college students’ critical thinking: a review of
    studies. Research in Higher Education, 26, 3e29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
    BF00991931.

    Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., & Howerter, A. (2000). The
    unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex
    ’frontal lobe’ tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49e100.
    http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734.

    Moutier, S., Angeard, N., & Houdé, O. (2002). Deductive reasoning and matching-
    bias inhibition training: evidence from a debiasing paradigm. Thinking and
    Reasoning, 8, 205e224. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546780244000033.

    Moutier, S., & Houdé, O. (2003). Judgement under uncertainty and conjunction
    fallacy inhibition training. Thinking and Reasoning, 9, 185e201. http://dx.doi.org/
    10.1080/13546780343000213.

    Nisbett, R. E., Fong, G. T., Lehman, D. R., & Cheng, P. W. (1987). Teaching reasoning.
    Science, 238, 625e631. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.3672116.

    Paas, F. (1992). Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem solving skills in
    statistics: a cognitive-load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 429e
    434. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.84.4.429.

    Renkl, A. (1997). Learning from worked-out examples: a study on individual differ-
    ences. Cognitive Science, 21, 1e29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2101_1.

    Renkl, A. (2002). Worked-out examples: instructional explanations support
    learning by self-explanations. Learning and Instruction, 12, 529e556. http://
    dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4752(01)00030-5.

    Renkl, A. (2005). The worked-out principle in multi-media learning. In R. E. Mayer
    (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 229e246). Cam-
    bridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Ritchart, R., & Perkins, D. N. (2005). Learning to think: the challenges of teaching
    thinking. In K. J. Holyoak, & R. G. Morrison (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of
    thinking and reasoning (pp. 775e802). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Rittle-Johnson, B. (2006). Promoting transfer: effects of self-explanation and direct
    instruction. Child Development, 77, 1e15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
    8624.2006.00852.x.

    Roy, M., & Chi, M. T. (2005). The self-explanation principle in multimedia learning.
    In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 271e
    286). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Sa, W., Kelley, C., Ho, C., & Stanovich, K. E. (2005). Thinking about personal theories:
    individual differences in the coordination of theory and evidence. Personality
    and Individual Differences, 38, 1149e1161. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004.
    07.012.

    Sa, W. C., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (1999). The domain specificity and generality of
    belief bias: searching for a generalizable critical thinking skill. Journal of Educa-
    tional Psychology, 91, 497e510. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.497.

    Sanfey, A. G., Loewenstein, G., McClure, S. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2006). Neuro-
    economics: cross-currents in resaerch on decision-making. Trends in Cognitive
    Sciences, 10, 108e116. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.009.

    Schagen, I., & Elliot, K. (2004). But what does it mean? The use of effect sizes in
    educational research. London: National Foundation for Educational Research.

    Smith, G. F. (2003). Beyond critical thinking and decision making: teaching business
    students how to think. Journal of Management Education, 27, 24e51. http://
    dx.doi.org/10.1177/1052562902239247.

    Stanovich, K. E. (2009). Distinguishing the reflective algorithmic and autonomous
    minds: is it time for a tri-process theory? In J. Evans, & K. Frankish (Eds.), In two
    minds: Dual processess and beyond (pp. 55e88) Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Stanovich, K. E. (2011). Rationality & the reflective mind. New York: Oxford Univer-
    sity Press.

    Stanovich, K. E., & Stanovich, P. J. (2010). A frame-work for critical thinking, rational
    thinking and intelligence. In D. D. Preiss, & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Educational
    psychology; Perspectives on learning, teaching and human development (pp. 195e
    237). New York: Springer Publishing Company.

    Stanovich, K. E., Toplak, M. E., & West, R. F. (2008). The development of rational
    thought: a taxonomy of heuristic and biases. Advances in Child Development and
    Behavior, 36, 251e285. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0065-2407(08)000062.

    Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1997). Reasoning independently of prior belief and
    individual differences in actively open-minded thinking. Journal of Educational
    Psychology, 89, 342e357. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.89.2.342.

    Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2007). Natural myside bias is independent of
    cognitive ability. Thinking and Reasoning, 13, 225e247. http://dx.doi.org/
    10.1080/13546780600780796.

    Ten Dam, G., & Volman, M. (2004). Critical thinking as a citizenship competence:
    teaching strategies. Learning and Instruction, 14, 359e379. http://dx.doi.org/
    10.1016/j.learninstruc.2004.01.005.

    Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of
    choice. Science, 211, 453e458. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.7455683.

    Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: the
    conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review, 90, 293e315.
    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.90.4.293.

    Van Gog, T., & Paas, F. (2008). Instructional efficiency: revisiting the original
    construct in educational research. Educational Psychologist, 43, 16e26. http://
    dx.doi.org/10.1080/00461520701756248.

    Wason, P. C., & Shapiro, D. (1971). Natural and contrived experience in a reasoning
    problem. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 23, 63e71. http://
    dx.doi.org/10.1080/00335557143000068.

    Wasserman, E. A., Dorner, W. W., & Kao, S. F. (1990). Contributions of specific cell
    information to judgments of interevent contingency. Journal of Experimental
    Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 509e521. http://dx.doi.org/
    10.1037/0278-7393.16.3.509.

    Wenzlaff, R. M., & Bates, D. E. (2000). The relative efficacy of concentration and
    suppression strategies of mental control. Personality and Social Psychology
    Bulletin, 26, 1200e1212. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167200262003.

    West, R. F., Toplak, M. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (2008). Heuristics and biases as measures
    of critical thinking: associations with cognitive ability and thinking disposi-
    tions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 930e941. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
    a0012842.

    Williams, J. J., & Lombrozo, T. (2010). The role of explanation in discovery and
    generalization: evidence from category learning. Cognitive Science, 34, 776e806.
    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01113.x.

    Wolcott, S. K., Baril, C. P., Cunningham, B. M., Fordham, D. R., & St.Pierre, K. (2002).
    Critical thought on critical thinking research. Journal of Accounting Education,
    20, 85e103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0748-5751(01)00029-X.

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref6

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref6

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref6

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.41.10.1078

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.41.10.1078

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.5.938

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.002

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.002

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.119.2.197

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.119.2.197

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4803_13

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4803_13

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.08.012

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093629

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093629

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2011.07.007

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2011.07.007

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref16

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref16

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref16

    http://www.insightassessment.com/9articles%2520WW.html

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(86)90001-0

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(86)90001-0

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.53.4.449

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/tl.8005

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0022370

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0022370

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref22

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref22

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref22

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0273475307302015

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00243.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00243.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-66X.43.6.431

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-66X.43.6.431

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref26

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.08.004

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2007.05.003

    http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03199552

    http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03199552

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.08.004

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00991931

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00991931

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546780244000033

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546780343000213

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546780343000213

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.3672116

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.84.4.429

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2101_1

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4752(01)00030-5

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4752(01)00030-5

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref39

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref39

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref39

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref39

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref40

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref40

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref40

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref40

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00852.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00852.x

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref42

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref42

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref42

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004.07.012

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004.07.012

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.497

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.009

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref46

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref46

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1052562902239247

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1052562902239247

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref48

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref48

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref48

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref48

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref49

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref49

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref50

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref50

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref50

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S0959-4752(13)00058-3/sref50

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0065-2407(08)000062

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.89.2.342

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546780600780796

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546780600780796

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2004.01.005

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2004.01.005

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.7455683

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.90.4.293

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00461520701756248

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00461520701756248

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00335557143000068

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00335557143000068

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.16.3.509

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.16.3.509

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167200262003

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0012842

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0012842

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01113.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0748-5751(01)00029-X

    • Improving critical thinking: Effects of dispositions and instructions on economics students’ reasoning skills
    • 1 Introduction
      1.1 Critical thinking
      1.2 Critical thinking instruction: avoiding biased reasoning
      1.2.1 Self-explanation prompts
      1.2.2 Activation prompts
      1.3 The present study
      2 Method
      2.1 Participants and design
      2.2 Materials and procedure
      2.2.1 Critical thinking disposition tests
      2.2.2 Critical thinking skills tests
      2.2.3 Critical thinking instructions
      2.2.4 Practice phase
      2.2.5 Procedure
      2.2.6 Data analysis

      3 Results
      3.1 Dispositions and pre-test performance
      3.2 Effects of instructions and dispositions on immediate and delayed post-test performance
      4 Discussion
      Appendix A Example of each category of tasks used on critical thinking tests (*=right option; 1=category practiced; 2=categ …
      References

    H

    E

    a

    b

    a

    A

    R
    R

    A
    A

    K
    C
    C
    C
    L
    L

    1

    c

    W

    i

    o
    i

    M

    c
    e

    1

    Thinking Skills and Creativity 21 (2016) 41–49

    Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

    Thinking Skills and Creativity

    j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : h t t p : / / w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / t s c

    ow might language affect critical thinking performance?

    mmanuel Manalo a,∗, Chris Sheppard b

    Graduate School of Education, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan
    Center for English Language Education, Faculty of Science and Engineering, Waseda University, Tokyo, Japan

    r t i c l e i n f o

    rticle history:
    eceived 27 July 2015
    eceived in revised form 29 March 2016
    ccepted 11 May 2016
    vailable online 12 May 2016

    eywords:
    ritical thinking skills instruction
    ritical evaluation
    ognitive cost
    anguage proficiency
    anguage structure

    a b s t r a c t

    This study examined whether language structure or language proficiency might affect
    students’ critical thinking performance. Previous research has claimed that many non-
    Western students struggle with the demands of demonstrating critical thought. Two
    language-related causes have been suggested: one concerning structural limitations in the
    non-Western students’ first language, and the other concerning their second language pro-
    ficiency. In Study 1 described here, reports written by 110 Japanese second year university
    students, who had received instruction in academic discourse for critical evaluation (which
    is one aspect of critical thinking), were analyzed for use of evaluative statements. No dis-
    advantage was found for use of the Japanese language, which is considered as having a
    more indirect structure that may make critical evaluation more difficult. Measurements of
    language proficiency in English and Japanese, however, were found to correlate with pro-
    duction of evaluative statements in those respective languages suggesting that language
    proficiency could affect critical evaluation use. In Study 2, the same task was given to 4

    3

    first year students who had not yet received the same instruction. Analysis revealed similar
    patterns in their written work but at a lower level, suggesting that the second year students
    had benefitted from the skills instruction. Furthermore, unlike the second year students,
    the first year students evidenced no correlations between their language proficiency scores
    and their production of evaluative statements, suggesting that proficiency on its own is
    inadequate: students need instruction on the specific language forms and structures to use
    to demonstrate critical thinking in their written work.

    © 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

    . Introduction

    In modern societies, the ability to evaluate the credibility of information that one encounters is an essential skill, especially
    onsidering the proliferation of unvetted information through the Internet and other forms of mass media (e.g., Glassner,

    einstock, & Neuman, 2005; Thomm & Bromme, 2011). However, educational development of critical thinking skills, which
    ncludes critical evaluation, is not a straightforward matter. There are, for example, disagreements about appropriate meth-
    ds for critical thinking skills instruction and assessment, and various individual and situational factors are believed to
    nfluence the development and use of such skills (e.g., Davies, 2006; Halpern, 1998; Ku, 2009; Manalo, Kusumi, Koyasu,

    ichita, & Tanaka, 2013, 2015; Ten Dam & Volman, 2004). One of the main areas of contention is whether cultural background
    ontributes to differences in critical thinking performance (e.g., Ennis, 1998; Manalo et al., 2013, 2015). Some authors, for
    xample, have portrayed Asian students as being deficient in critical thinking compared to Western students (e.g., Atkinson,

    ∗ Corresponding author at: Graduate School of Education, Kyoto University, Yoshida-Honmachi, Sakyo-ku, Kyoto 606-8501, Japan.
    E-mail address: manalo.emmanuel.3z@kyoto-u.ac.jp (E. Manalo).

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2016.05.005
    871-1871/© 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

    dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2016.05.005

    http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/18711871

    http://www.elsevier.com/locate/tsc

    http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1016/j.tsc.2016.05.005&domain=pdf

    mailto:manalo.emmanuel.3z@kyoto-u.ac.jp

    dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2016.05.005

    42 E. Manalo, C. Sheppard / Thinking Skills and Creativity 21 (2016) 41–49

    1997; Fox, 1994), and it has been found that many instructors at the tertiary level subscribe to such a view (e.g., Lee &
    Carrasquillo, 2006; Robertson, Line, Jones, & Thomas, 2000).

    Two language-related explanations have been proposed for the apparent differences in critical thinking performance
    manifested by students from different cultural backgrounds. One of those explanations concerns the structure of the student’s
    native language (L1). This explanation posits that, due to their structure, some languages may present constraints in the ease
    with which certain thinking skills can be undertaken or expressed. This explanation is sometimes referred to as the “Sapir-
    Whorf hypothesis” or the “linguistic relativity hypothesis” (see, e.g., Au, 1983; Hill & Mannheim, 1992; Hockett, 1954; Whorf,
    1956). An example of a claim of this kind is Bloom’s (1981) proposal that counterfactual thinking (i.e., thinking about what
    might have been, contrary to facts) may be more difficult in Chinese compared to English.

    Some observations of linguistic differences, such as “indirectness” being a feature more prevalent in some languages,
    particularly Asian languages (e.g., Kong, 2005), would appear to support the idea that language structure could affect the ease
    with which certain modes of thinking could be undertaken or expressed. In a study by Itakura and Tsui (2011), for example,
    evidence was found that book reviewers use different strategies to convey critical evaluation when writing in Japanese
    compared to English: in Japanese, criticism is usually indirectly conveyed and is frequently preceded by an apology. Previous
    studies, however, have failed to provide convincing evidence that language structures could actually impose constraints in
    what users of the language can communicate. Although the earlier-mentioned study by Bloom (1981) claimed to have found
    evidence for such constraints where counterfactual thinking in the Chinese language is concerned, subsequent investigations
    failed to replicate or support Bloom’s results (Au, 1983; Liu, 1985; Takano, 1989; Vorster & Schuring, 1989). Even where
    the weaker version of the linguistic relativity hypothesis is concerned (i.e., that language can merely influence people’s
    thought patterns, rather than constrain their thought processes), the findings of the few studies that claim to have found
    supporting evidence (e.g., Boroditsky, 2001) have not been replicated in subsequent independent investigations (e.g., Chen,
    2007; January & Kako, 2007; Tse & Altarriba, 2008).

    The other language-related explanation for the apparent culture-based differences in critical thinking performance con-
    cerns students’ second language proficiency (e.g., Floyd, 2011; Lun, Fischer, & Ward, 2010; Paton, 2005). This explanation is
    based on the observation that the students who are usually reported as having lower levels of critical thinking competence
    are international students in English speaking countries. It suggests that, because most of those students have to use a sec-
    ond language (L2, which is usually English) in their host environment, and they may lack adequate proficiency in that L2,
    they would likely manifest lower competence when performing tasks that require use of that L2. Tasks that are likely to get
    affected include cognitive tasks like critical thinking.

    One way of understanding this possible influence of language proficiency on critical thinking performance is in terms
    of cognitive cost (i.e., the mental resources cost associated with task performance). Language processing entails the use of
    cognitive resources in working memory (Baddeley, 1986, 1998), and lower proficiency in a language would require the use
    of more resources. Thinking critically would likewise require the use of working memory resources. There are, however,
    limited resources available in working memory (Baddeley, 1986, 1998) and, if a considerable amount of those resources has
    already been expended on utilizing a language in which proficiency is low, there may not be adequate resources remaining
    for the satisfactory execution of critical thinking.

    The negative impact of the higher cognitive cost entailed in using a language in which proficiency is low, on the execution
    of other cognitive tasks, has been demonstrated in previous research. Takano and Noda (1993, 1995) showed that the use
    of a foreign language detrimentally affects performance in concurrently undertaken non-linguistic tasks like arithmetic
    calculation and mental imagery, and Manalo and Uesaka (2012, 2014) reported evidence indicating that students’ lower
    proficiency in an L2 limits their ability to use diagrams when explaining information in that L2. Where critical thinking
    is concerned, both Lun et al. (2010) and Floyd (2011) reported indications that lower proficiency levels in English could
    detrimentally affect Asian students’ performance in critical thinking tests administered in English. However, neither of those
    studies used appropriate, objective measures of L2 proficiency to reliably confirm the connection between L2 proficiency
    and critical thinking skills performance.

    1.1. Overview of the present study

    The present study examined whether there might be evidence to support either or both (i) the language structure expla-
    nation, and/or (ii) the language proficiency explanation, in students’ manifestation of critical thinking in their written work.
    The study was not intended to be a comprehensive test of the language structure hypothesis: it examined only whether, in
    the written work of Japanese university students, there might be observable differences in the presence of critical thinking
    qualities depending on the language used, Japanese or English. Critical thinking was operationalized as students’ use of
    evaluative statements. This decision was based on the fact that use of evaluative statements comprises a salient expression
    of critical evaluation, which in turn is central to the notion of critical thinking application (e.g., Fisher & Scriven’s, 1997, p.
    21, definition of critical thinking as “skilled and active interpretation and evaluation of observations and communications,
    information and argumentation” – italics added).

    In the present study, Japanese was deemed an appropriate language to examine because, like a number of other Asian
    languages, it employs patterns of expression that make it more indirect and inductive compared to English (e.g., Itakura &
    Tsui, 2011; Scollon & Wong-Scollon, 1991). Evaluation, however, requires precision and directness in conveying judgments
    about the quality or value of the subject being referred to. Thus, structural features of the Japanese language could make

    t
    l

    m

    O

    r
    t
    w
    a
    b
    t
    a
    h
    b

    e
    t

    a

    h
    t
    p
    r
    l
    n
    t

    u
    i
    a
    l
    s
    l
    t
    a

    2

    E
    o

    2
    2

    F
    c
    d

    2

    e
    r
    c

    t
    w
    a
    t

    E. Manalo, C. Sheppard / Thinking Skills and Creativity 21 (2016) 41–49 43

    he production of evaluative language relatively more difficult. If so, it should be possible to detect lower rates of evaluative
    anguage use in the students’ written work in Japanese compared to English.

    It was equally important to consider whether using an L2 might detrimentally affect students’ critical evaluation perfor-
    ance. If this was the case, one would expect that critical evaluation performance in the L1 would exceed that in the L2.
    ne would also expect to find a relationship between the critical evaluation performance and L2 proficiency. Thus, possible

    elationships between students’ L2 proficiency and their production of evaluative statements were investigated. The ques-
    ion was whether L2 proficiency would manifest as a limiting factor because lower proficiency entails higher cognitive cost
    hen using the L2, leaving insufficient resources in working memory for critical evaluation. If this explanation is supported,

    relationship should be found between the students’ L2 proficiency and their evaluative statements production in the L2,
    ut not in the L1. A relationship in both the L1 and the L2 would suggest that general language or intellectual abilities – rather
    han L2 proficiency – affect critical evaluation performance. The reason is that language abilities, and intellectual abilities
    nd performance, are generally considered as being related (e.g., Ackerman, 1986; Neisser et al., 1996). Thus, a student with
    igher language and intellectual abilities could be expected to score higher in measurements of L2 proficiency, and evidence
    etter performance in tasks like critical evaluation – in both their L1 and L2.

    Furthermore, it is conceivable that this kind of relationship between language proficiency and critical evaluation also
    xists in the L1. In other words, that the more proficient or skilled a student might be in using his or her native language,
    he less cognitive resources such use might demand, and the more resources might be made available for critical evaluation

    which could then be better executed. Thus, in the present study, the possible existence of this relationship in the L1 was
    lso investigated.

    When considering the development of critical evaluation skills through instruction, if the language structure explanation
    olds, one would expect that skills instruction provided in the L2 would not result in any discernible skills improvement in
    he L1 (i.e., since the L1 has inherent structural constraints on the use or expression of those skills). In contrast, if the language
    roficiency explanation holds, one could expect that skills instruction provided in the L2 – where proficiency is lower – would
    esult in at least some discernable transfer to the L1, where proficiency is higher and more cognitive processing resources are
    ikely to be available for the application of those skills. Critical evaluation skills are thinking skills and its acquisition should
    ot be bound to the language of instruction. The important question is whether constraints or limitations in the language
    o use could detrimentally affect critical evaluation performance.

    The research reported here comprised two related studies. In Study 1, evaluative statements that second year Japanese
    niversity students produced in Japanese (their L1) and in English (their L2) were examined. These students had received

    nstruction on academic discourse. Thus, they were not naïve as to the requirements of expressing evaluative language,
    nd any differences in the writing they produced in L1 and L2 could be attributed to either the inherent structure of the
    anguage they were using or their proficiency in using that language. In Study 2, the same writing task was given to first year
    tudents who had received little instruction on academic discourse, and nothing explicit on the production of evaluative
    anguage. The purpose of this second study was to find out if the characteristics of L1 and L2 written work produced by
    he first year students, compared to their second year counterparts, differed. Hence, the aim was to find out whether the
    dditional instruction that had been received by the more advanced second year students might have made a difference.

    . Study 1

    Study 1 tested two hypotheses. The first was that students’ production of evaluative statements in Japanese and in
    nglish would differ. The second was that the students’ proficiency in a particular language would be related to the amount
    f evaluative statements they produce in that language.

    .1. Method

    .1.1. Participants
    The participants were 110 Japanese university students in their second year of study in science and engineering disciplines.

    or these students, Japanese is L1 and English is L2. They were taking a compulsory English communication skills development
    ourse that covers oral and written academic discourse using a task-based learning approach. The students came from four
    ifferent classes in that course.

    .1.2. Materials and procedure
    As part of the communication skills course, the students were provided class instruction, textbook explanations and

    xamples (Anthony, Rose, & Sheppard, 2010), and practice in the use of language appropriate for critical evaluation, including
    anking and debating different reasons and other forms of alternatives (e.g., clearly stating the premises, and then drawing
    onclusions). These materials and the instruction were all provided in English.

    For the purposes of the present investigation, the students were additionally provided with a single page Japanese

    ranslation of the part of the textbook dealing with how to make valid arguments. They were also supplied brief (one page)
    ritten examples (one in English and one in Japanese) of how alternative reasons could be ranked according to judgments

    bout their relative importance. The example texts conveyed someone’s opinion about the most important reason for learning
    he English language, among four possible reasons. The texts provided examples of evaluative statements and provision of

    44 E. Manalo, C. Sheppard / Thinking Skills and Creativity 21 (2016) 41–49

    support for claims, although those were not labeled or overtly identified in any way in the texts. The equivalence and
    appropriate use of language in the English and Japanese versions were checked by several bilingual teachers of the course.
    Although all materials provided in the course are usually in English, the Japanese versions were supplied in this case to avoid
    possible disadvantage to the students’ production of evaluative language in Japanese (i.e., without the Japanese versions, it
    could be argued that the students might have simply been unfamiliar with the equivalent Japanese expressions for critical
    evaluation).

    During two consecutive 90-min class sessions of the course, the students were introduced to the Titanic and Space Shuttle
    Challenger disasters, including four basic causes that have been proposed for the occurrence of each of those disasters. During
    the class sessions, the students participated in guided exercises to explore and discuss the disasters and their corresponding
    possible causes.

    For homework, the students were asked to write two brief reports to explain what they considered to be the most
    important cause of each of the disasters. To avoid any possible misunderstandings about the requirements of the homework
    task, written instructions were provided in Japanese. The students were randomly assigned to write one report in English
    and the other in Japanese (i.e., if they were asked to write the Titanic report in English, they had to write the Challenger
    report in Japanese, and vice versa).

    2.1.3. Analyses
    The total number of sentences [Total], and the number of evaluative sentences (i.e., sentences where some evaluation

    of the relative value of the topic is made) [Evaluative], were counted and scored in the analysis of the students’ written
    work.

    Operational criteria were drawn up for determining what data counted as “evaluative sentences”. Examples of such
    requirements included: the sentence must explicitly say something about the worth or value of the subject, and that worth
    or value must be in comparison to something else. Conditional statements that explicitly convey a relative evaluation of the
    subject were counted. The following examples, in contrast, did not count: the use of simple adjectives or adverbs to describe
    something, prescriptive statements not explicitly expressing a relative evaluation or judgment, and conditional statements
    in general. Inter-rater reliability was checked by asking an independent coder to score a randomly selected sample of 25% of
    the data. Reliability coefficients obtained (Cronbach’s alphas) were deemed to be satisfactory: they were 0.922 in English,
    and 0.960 in Japanese.

    Language proficiency was measured (i) by using the students’ TOEIC-IP (Test of English for International Communication
    – Institution Program) scores, and (ii) in terms of the complexity of the sentences the students produced in English and in
    Japanese (explained below). TOEIC is a norm-referenced test of English listening comprehension and reading skills, widely
    used as a measure of students’ English language proficiency levels in Japan (http://www.ets.org/toeic). Although there is
    a speaking and writing test version, it is not as widely used as the listening and reading test version, which is what the
    TOEIC-IP contains. Students in the faculty where the students came from are required to sit the TOEIC-IP test at regular
    intervals during their period of enrolment, and their scores on that test are made available to their teachers. The TOEIC test
    has been used in numerous previous studies as a measure of L2 proficiency (e.g., Dornyei & Katona, 1992; Eggly, Musial,
    & Smulowitz, 1999), and various studies have reported that it has at least adequate reliability and validity (e.g., In’nami
    & Koizumi, 2011; Perkins, 1987; Powers, Kim, & Weng, 2008). The TOEIC is also considered a good measure of non-native
    English speakers’ ability to use English in real life situations rather than just in strictly academic contexts (Eggly et al., 1999).
    For these reasons, the authors of the present study decided to use the students’ TOEIC-IP scores as one measurement of their
    English proficiency.

    The other measure of proficiency used – complexity – was selected because, along with accuracy and fluency, it is
    generally considered as one of the key dimensions of proficiency in language production – both oral and written (e.g.,
    Ellis, 2009; Ishikawa, 1995; Ortega, 2003; Skehan & Foster, 1999; Wolfe-Quintero, Inagaki, & Kim, 1998). However, unlike
    accuracy and fluency, for which it is difficult to come up with meaningful and equivalent/comparable measurements
    for English and Japanese, the authors of the present study were able to determine a viable way to measure complexity.
    Usually, analysis of production complexity in English entails calculating the ratio of words over sentences or terminable
    units (T-units; e.g., Ishikawa, 1995; Ortega, 2003; Richards, Platt, & Platt, 1992). However, what counts as a word in
    Japanese is not exactly the same as in English, and thus, such a method for calculating complexity was deemed inap-
    propriate. Instead, the number of verbs that the students used were counted and divided by their total number of
    sentences to generate a measure of complexity. Verbs were chosen as they are comparable in English and Japanese,
    and the use of more verbs can be considered a good indicator of more complex sentence structures (i.e., simple sen-
    tences usually contain only one verb, and inclusion of more verbs within one sentence usually requires the use of a
    more complex structure). It should also be noted that even though this measurement of complexity and the measure-
    ment of the students’ use of critical evaluation were derived from the same source (i.e., the writing that the students
    produced), these measurements were not dependent on each other. In other words, students can produce complex
    writing without being critically evaluative, and vice versa: they can write evaluative sentences that are low in complex-

    ity.

    Analyses of variance were conducted to compare the students’ production of sentences (Total, and Evaluative) in English
    and in Japanese. Correlational analyses were carried out to examine possible relationships with the students’ most recent
    TOEIC test scores and their complexity scores in English and in Japanese.

    http://www.ets.org/toeic

    E. Manalo, C. Sheppard / Thinking Skills and Creativity 21 (2016) 41–49 45

    Table 1
    Means (with standard deviations in brackets) of total and evaluative sentences that the second year students produced in English and in Japanese.

    Total Evaluative

    English 20.31 (5.59) 3.46 (1.76)
    Japanese 18.68 (5.92) 3.75 (1.80)

    Table 2
    Correlation coefficients between second year students’ TOEIC and writing complexity scores, and numbers and proportions of evaluative sentences they
    produced, according to the language used (effect sizes, R2 shown in brackets).

    Scores English Evaluative Sentences Japanese Evaluative Sentences

    Number Proportion Number Proportion

    TOEIC 0.23* (0.053) 0.14 (0.020) 0.18 (0.032) −0.06 (0.004)
    English Complexity 0.19* (0.036) 0.24* (0.058) 0.15 (0.023) 0.17 (0.029)
    Japanese Complexity 0.02 (0.000) 0.09 (0.008) −0.07 (0.005) 0.33*** (0.109)

    *

    *
    *
    2
    i

    c
    e
    t

    m
    J
    l
    1
    t

    p
    t
    i
    t

    E
    t
    b
    o
    c
    s
    o
    b
    t

    E
    e

    2

    E
    h
    w

    l
    i

    p < 0.05. * p < 0.01. ** p < 0.001.

    .2. Results

    Table 1 shows the means, and standard deviations (in brackets), obtained for Total and Evaluative categories of sentences
    n the students’ written work in English and in Japanese.

    Repeated measures 2 × 2 ANOVAs demonstrated that there were no significant effects due to the task (i.e., the Titanic
    ompared to the Challenger reports), hence demonstrating task equivalence. The analysis however revealed significant
    ffects due to language in both the total number of sentences written [Total], F(1, 108) = 11.47, p = 0.001, �p2 = 0.020; and
    he number of evaluative sentences [Evaluative], F(1, 108) = 4.81, p = 0.030, �p2 = 0.006.

    These results indicate that the students wrote significantly more sentences in English compared to Japanese, but produced
    ore of the target evaluative language in Japanese. The ANOVA results also revealed that the students wrote more verbs in

    apanese (mean = 48.52, SD = 14.01) than in English (mean = 38.77, SD = 12.54), F(1, 109) = 84.56, p < 0.001, �p2 = 0.119. The anguage produced was also significantly more complex in Japanese as measured by the number of verbs per sentence (F(1, 09) = 100.8, p < 0.001, �p2 = 0.269), indicating that the reason for the production of fewer sentences in Japanese was because he students’ greater proficiency in that language enabled them to produce more complex sentences.

    Because the total number of sentences that the students produced differed considerably between the two languages, the
    roportions of evaluative sentences (i.e., Evaluative as a proportion of Total) were also calculated and compared according
    o the language used. The comparison revealed a significant difference: F(1, 109) = 20.32, p < 0.001, �p2 = 0.039. This result ndicates that the proportion of evaluative sentences was higher in the reports that the students wrote in Japanese compared o those they wrote in English.

    For the correlational analysis, the correlations between the students’ TOEIC scores and their writing complexity scores in
    nglish and in Japanese, and the evaluative sentences they produced in the respective languages (actual and proportions of
    otal), were examined. The results are shown in Table 2. The students’ TOEIC scores correlated significantly with the num-
    er of evaluative sentences they produced in English. The correlation between the students’ TOEIC scores and the number
    f evaluative sentences they produced in Japanese was not significant. Where the complexity scores were concerned, the
    omplexity of the students’ writing in English significantly correlated with both the number and proportion of evaluative
    entences they produced in English. The complexity of their writing in Japanese significantly correlated with the proportion
    f evaluative sentences they produced in Japanese. All cross language correlations between writing complexity and num-
    ers/proportions of evaluative sentences were not significant. Taken together, these results lend support to the argument
    hat language proficiency influences students’ ability to evidence critical evaluation in their writing.

    A significant correlation was also found between the proportions of evaluative sentences the students produced in
    nglish and in Japanese, r = 0.72, p < 0.001, R2 (effect size) = 0.518. This result suggests that students who were more critically valuative in one language also demonstrated greater critical evaluation in the other language.

    .3. Discussion

    Differences were found in both actual numbers and proportions of evaluative sentences that the students produced in
    nglish and in Japanese. The direction of the differences, however, was opposite to the language structure-based prediction:
    igher proportions of evaluative sentences were found in Japanese instead of English. This result suggests that the students

    ere better at producing evaluative language in their L1.

    The significant relationship found between the students’ language proficiency and their critical evaluation performance
    ends support to the notion that language proficiency could explain apparent cultural differences in students’ critical think-
    ng performance. When using an L2 (such as English) in which they may lack adequate proficiency (e.g., less automated

    46 E. Manalo, C. Sheppard / Thinking Skills and Creativity 21 (2016) 41–49

    processing, inadequate linguistic knowledge), Asian students in Western countries may not be able to demonstrate critical
    thinking to the same degree as their native English-speaker counterparts because of the higher cognitive processing resource
    requirements of that L2, which depletes resources available in working memory for critical thinking. The important find-
    ing in the present study is that proficiency in both languages (English and Japanese) significantly correlated with critical
    evaluation performance in the respective language.

    There still remains the possibility that the significant correlations between the two languages in the proportions of the
    target language produced, are a result of other factors, which are correlated to both first and second language proficiency,
    and which might affect the production of evaluative language – irrespective of the language being used. In other words, in
    addition to language proficiency, people’s critical evaluation performance may also be influenced by some other individual
    factors – such as general intelligence, working memory capacity, or communication skills – that are independent of the
    language being used for expression. It would be useful to identify and examine those other factors in future research.

    As noted earlier, the student participants in this first study had already received instruction in academic discourse that
    included the use of evaluative language. Therefore, an important next question to address was, “To what extent had that
    instruction affected the relative production of evaluative language in English and in Japanese?” – which was pursued in
    Study 2.

    3. Study 2

    Study 2 tested three hypotheses. The first was that first year students would manifest lower use of the target evalua-
    tive language compared to the second year students from whom data were gathered in Study 1. The second was that the
    differences in evaluative language use would be consistent across English and Japanese. In other words, the prediction was
    that, like the second year students, the first year students would evidence greater use of evaluative statements in their L1
    (Japanese) compared to their L2 (English). The third hypothesis was that the first year students’ proficiency in both languages
    would not be related to the proportions of evaluative language they produce. This hypothesis was based on the assumption
    that, without receiving instruction about how to undertake and convey critical evaluation, students’ language proficiency
    would not make a difference to their production of the target evaluative language as they would have very limited knowledge
    about the structures necessary for producing the required language.

    3.1. Method

    3.1.1. Participants
    Forty-three Japanese university students who were in their first year of studies in the same science and engineering

    faculty as the students in Study 1 were added to the participants of Study 1, for a total of 153 participants. The additional
    students came from two classes of a compulsory first year English communication skills course which deals with various
    aspects of oral and written academic discourse, but nothing explicit about evaluative language (which is not covered until
    the second year course).

    3.1.2. Materials, procedure, and analysis
    For one of their homework assignments, the students were given brief reading materials (in English and in Japanese)

    about the Titanic and Space Shuttle Challenger disasters, including the proposed causes of those disasters. These materials
    were drawn from the textbook used in the second year course. The Japanese translations were provided to these first year
    participants to ensure that their subsequent writing performance would not have been compromised by possible difficulties
    in understanding the English versions. The contents of those materials were not covered in class.

    The homework task that the students had to do was the same as that given to the second year students: to produce
    two brief reports to explain what they considered to be the most important cause of each of the disasters, after reading the
    materials provided. Like the second year students, they were randomly assigned to write one report in English and the other
    in Japanese. Also, like the second year students, they were provided with the one-page examples (one in English and one in
    Japanese) of how alternative reasons (for learning the English language) could be ranked according to judgments about their
    relative importance. The crucial difference was that the first year students were not provided class instruction and exercises
    on the use of academic discourse specifically pertaining to evaluative language.

    The written reports that the students produced were analyzed and scored in the same manner described in the first study.
    The first and second year students’ data were then compared using repeated measures ANOVAs. Correlational analyses were
    carried out to examine possible relationships between the students’ language proficiency scores and the proportions of
    evaluative language they produced.

    3.2. Results

    Table 3 shows the means, and standard deviations (in brackets), obtained for the Total and Evaluative categories of
    sentences in the first year students’ written work in English and in Japanese.

    Analyses of variance revealed significant effects due to year of enrolment (first year compared to second year) in the stu-
    dents’ scores for the total number of sentences [Total], F(1, 152) = 22.90, p < 0.001, �p2 = 0.113; and the number of evaluative

    E. Manalo, C. Sheppard / Thinking Skills and Creativity 21 (2016) 41–49 47

    Table 3
    Means (with standard deviations in brackets) of total and evaluative sentences that the first year students produced in English and in Japanese.

    Total Evaluative

    English 15.70 (8.20) 2.09 (1.71)
    Japanese 12.84 (6.28) 2.26 (1.24)

    Table

    4

    Correlation coefficients between first year students’ TOEIC and writing complexity scores, and proportions of evaluative sentences they produced, according
    to the language used (effect sizes, R2 shown in brackets).

    Scores Proportions of Evaluative Sentences

    English Japanese

    s
    1
    s

    f
    m
    t
    a
    i
    fi
    E

    w
    t

    3

    o
    l
    c
    s
    t
    p

    i
    p
    i
    i

    4

    p
    s
    fi
    f
    a
    r

    l
    o
    r
    s
    t

    TOEIC 0.03 (0.000) −0.02 (0.000)
    English Complexity 0.16 (0.026) −0.18 (0.032)
    Japanese Complexity 0.01 (0.000) 0.14 (0.020)

    entences [Evaluative], F(1, 152) = 27.20, p < 0.001, �p2 = 0.130. A significant language effects were also found for Total, F(1, 52) = 26.67, p < 0.001, �p2 = 0.026. No significant interaction effects between language and year were found; nor were any ignificant effects found due to the task (Titanic versus Challenger).

    These results indicate that, compared to the second year students, the first year students wrote fewer sentences in total
    or their reports, and fewer evaluative statements. Like the second year students, the first year students wrote significantly

    ore sentences in English than in Japanese. Analysis of the complexity of the sentences they produced revealed that the
    otal number of verbs they used was likewise higher in Japanese compared to English, F(1, 43) = 14.80, p < 0.001, �p2 = 0.032; nd the index of complexity (total number of verbs divided by total number of sentences) was higher for what they wrote n Japanese compared to what they wrote in English, F(1, 43) = 75.58, p < 0.001, �p2 = 0.409. This finding indicates that the rst year students, like their second year counterparts, were producing more complex sentences in Japanese compared to nglish.

    The results of the correlational analysis are shown in Table 4. None of the correlations were significant – suggesting that,
    ithout instruction, the students’ proficiency in both languages generally did not make a difference to their production of

    he target evaluative language.

    .3. Discussion

    The results of Study 2 showed that the second year students wrote more sentences in their reports, and produced more
    f the target evaluative language, compared to the first year students. This finding suggests that instruction on appropriate
    anguage to use – which had been provided to the second year students – can improve students’ abilities in manifesting
    ritical evaluation in their written work. Although as noted the instruction was provided almost entirely in English, the
    ignificant language effects found were all in favor of the Japanese language, which suggests that there is transfer across
    he languages in skills acquisition. In other words, skills taught and learned in English also produce improvements in the
    roduction of evaluative language in Japanese.

    The results of the correlational analysis underline the importance of providing instruction to cultivate student competence
    n critical evaluation. No significant correlations were found between the proportions of evaluative language the students
    roduced and their proficiency scores in English and in Japanese. This therefore suggests that general proficiency on its own

    s not adequate: for students to evidence critical evaluation in their written work, it is necessary to also provide them with
    nstruction on the specific language forms and structures to use.

    . General discussion

    The findings of this study provide evidence that, at least for Japanese students, using the Japanese language (their L1)
    resents no disadvantage compared to English (their L2) in the production of evaluative language (i.e., the Japanese language
    tructure is not a limiting factor). How Japanese students’ evaluative language use might compare to that of students whose
    rst language is structured differently (e.g., native English speakers responding to the same tasks), or students who are

    ully bilingual in Japanese and English, would need to be examined in future research. However, in the present study, there
    ppeared to be no obvious deficits in evaluative language production in Japanese among the second year students who had
    eceived instruction in the necessary academic discourse.

    There is evidence in the present study, however, that language proficiency can influence the production of evaluative
    anguage. Although the conceptualization of critical thinking is likely to be independent of the language being used, the use

    f a language that one is not so proficient in requires greater cognitive processing resources and thus limits the remaining
    esources that could be utilized for the expression of critical thinking. In this study, the significant correlations between the
    tudents’ English proficiency scores and the amount of evaluative language they evidenced in their English writing indicate
    hat performance varied with L2 proficiency. This provides useful evidence to corroborate previously made claims (e.g., Floyd,

    48 E. Manalo, C. Sheppard / Thinking Skills and Creativity 21 (2016) 41–49

    2011; Lun et al., 2010; Paton, 2005) that some of the shortcomings in critical thinking skills manifested by international
    students can be attributed to their having to use an L2 in which they may not be as proficient compared to their native
    speaker counterparts. Although those significant correlations were not high, results were also obtained in the present study
    showing that the students’ Japanese writing complexity significantly correlated with the proportion of evaluative language
    they produced in Japanese. This finding provides further support for the argument that language proficiency can influence
    the production of evaluative language, this time in the L1.

    4.1. Conclusion

    The finding that L2 proficiency is a potential limiting factor in students’ use of critical thinking skills suggests that
    educational strategies for improving non-native speaker students proficiencies in English (or the language used in the host
    country) would be beneficial. Successful implementation of such strategies would likely reduce the perceived deficiencies
    in Asian and other foreign students’ critical thinking competencies (cf. Lee & Carrasquillo, 2006; Robertson et al., 2000).
    The findings of this study also indicate that appropriate classroom instruction is essential for facilitating the development
    of students’ abilities to demonstrate critical thinking competencies in the work they produce. The second year students
    evidenced similar writing profiles to those of first year students; however, having received instructions in academic discourse
    relevant to critical evaluation, they also produced more of the target evaluative language. They did this in both languages,
    L1 and L2, even though academic discourse instruction was primarily provided in the L2 – suggesting some transfer of skills
    across languages. Furthermore, the lack of significant correlations between the first year students’ L1 and L2 proficiency and
    their use of the target evaluative language underline the necessity of providing critical evaluation instruction to students.
    Without receiving explicit instruction, most students − irrespective of their language proficiency – would not likely know
    how to effectively demonstrate critical evaluation in the work they produce.

    List of funder and grant number

    This research was supported by a grant-in-aid (23243071) received from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science.

    Conflict of interest

    There is no conflict of interest known to the authors.

    Acknowledgements

    The authors would like to thank Kyoko Watanabe and Rongting Shen for their invaluable assistance during various stages
    of this study.

    References

    Ackerman, P. L. (1986). Individual differences in information processing: an investigation of intellectual abilities and task performance during practice.
    Intelligence, 10, 101–139.

    Anthony, L., Rose, R., & Sheppard, C. (2010). Concept building and discussion: foundations. Tokyo: DTP Publishing.
    Atkinson, D. (1997). A critical approach to critical thinking in TESOL. TESOL Quarterly, 31, 9–37.
    Au, T. K. F. (1983). Chinese and English counterfactuals: the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis revisited. Cognition, 15, 155–187.
    Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    Baddeley, A. (1998). Human memory. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
    Bloom, A. (1981). The linguistic shaping of thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
    Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought?: Mandarin and English speakers’ conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 1–22.
    Chen, J. Y. (2007). Do Chinese and English speakers think about time differently? Failure of replicating Boroditsky (2001). Cognition, 104, 427–436.
    Davies, W. M. (2006). An ‘infusion’ approach to critical thinking: Moore on the critical thinking debate. Higher Education Research & Development, 25,

    179–193.
    Dornyei, Z., & Katona, L. (1992). Validation of the C-test amongst Hungarian EFL learners. Language Testing, 9, 187–206.
    Eggly, S., Musial, J., & Smulowitz, J. (1999). The relationship between English language proficiency and success as a medical resident. English for Specific

    Purposes, 18, 201–208.
    Ellis, R. (2009). The differential effects of three types of task planning on the fluency, complexity, and accuracy in L2 oral production. Applied Linguistics,

    30, 474–509.
    Ennis, R. H. (1998). Is critical thinking culturally biased? Teaching Philosophy, 21, 15–33.
    Fisher, A., & Scriven, M. (1997). Critical thinking: its definition and assessment. Norwich, UK: Centre for Research in Critical Thinking.
    Floyd, C. B. (2011). Critical thinking in a second language. Higher Education Research and Development, 30, 289–302.
    Fox, H. (1994). Listening to the world. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.
    Glassner, A., Weinstock, M., & Neuman, Y. (2005). Pupils’ evaluation and generation of evidence and explanation in argumentation. British Journal of

    Educational Psychology, 75, 105–118.
    Halpern, D. F. (1998). Teaching critical thinking for transfer across domains. American Psychologist, 53, 449–455.

    Hill, J. H., & Mannheim, B. (1992). Language and world view. Annual Review of Anthropology, 21, 381–406.
    Hockett, C. E. (1954). Chinese versus English: an exploration of the Whorfian thesis. In H. Hoijer (Ed.), Language in culture (pp. 106–123). Chicago:

    University of Chicago Press.
    In’nami, Y., & Koizumi, R. (2011). Factor structure of the revised TOEIC test: a multiple-sample analysis. Language Testing, 29, 131–152.
    Ishikawa, S. (1995). Objective measurement of low-proficiency EFL narrative writing. Journal of Second Language Writing, 4, 51–69.

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0030

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0030

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0030

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0030

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0030

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0030

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0030

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0035

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0035

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0035

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0035

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0035

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0035

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0035

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0035

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0035

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0085

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0085

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0085

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0085

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0085

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0085

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0085

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0085

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0085

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0085

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0085

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0085

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0085

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0115

    I

    J
    K
    K

    L
    L
    L

    M
    M
    M
    M

    N

    O

    P

    P
    P
    R
    R

    S
    S

    T

    T

    T
    T
    T

    T

    V

    W
    W

    E. Manalo, C. Sheppard / Thinking Skills and Creativity 21 (2016) 41–49 49

    takura, H., & Tsui, A. B. M. (2011). Evaluation in academic discourse: managing criticism in Japanese and English book reviews. Journal of Pragmatics, 43,
    1366–1379.

    anuary, D., & Kako, E. (2007). Re-evaluating evidence for the linguistic relativity hypothesis: response to Boroditsky (2001). Cognition, 104, 417–426.
    ong, K. C. C. (2005). Linguistic resources as evaluators in English and Chinese research articles. Multilingua, 24, 275–308.
    u, K. Y. L. (2009). Assessing students’ critical thinking performance: urging for measurements using multi-response format. Thinking Skills and Creativity,

    4, 70–76.
    ee, K. S., & Carrasquillo, A. (2006). Korean college students in United States: perceptions of professors and students. College Student Journal, 40, 442–456.
    iu, L. G. (1985). Reasoning counterfactually in Chinese: are there any obstacles? Cognition, 21, 239–270.
    un, V. M. C., Fischer, R., & Ward, C. (2010). Exploring cultural differences in critical thinking: is it about my thinking style or the language I speak?

    Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 604–616.
    analo, E., & Uesaka, Y. (2012). Elucidating the mechanism of spontaneous diagram use in explanations: how cognitive processing of text and

    diagrammatic representations is influenced by individual and task-related factors. In P. Cox, P. Rodgers, & B. Plimmer (Eds.), Diagrammatic
    representation and inference. Lecture notes in artificial intelligence (LNAI) 7352 (pp. 35–50). Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.

    analo, E., & Uesaka, Y. (2014). Students’ spontaneous use of diagrams in written communication: understanding variations according to purpose and
    cognitive cost entailed. In T. Dwyer, H. Purchase, & A. Delaney (Eds.), Diagrammatic representation and inference. Lecture notes in artificial intelligence
    (LNAI) 8578 (pp. 78–92). Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.

    analo, E., Kusumi, T., Koyasu, M., Michita, Y., & Tanaka, Y. (2013). To what extent do culture-related factors influence university students’ critical
    thinking use. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 10, 121–132.

    analo, E., Kusumi, T., Koyasu, M., Michita, Y., & Tanaka, Y. (2015). Do students from different cultures think differently about critical and other thinking
    skills? In M. Davies, & R. Barnett (Eds.), Palgrave handbook of critical thinking in higher education (pp. 299–316). New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

    eisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Boykin, A. W., Brody, N., Ceci, S. J., & Urbina, S. (1996). Intelligence: knowns and unknowns. American
    Psychologist, 51, 77–101.

    rtega, L. (2003). Syntactic complexity measures and their relationship to L2 proficiency: a research synthesis of college-level L2 writing. Applied
    Linguistics, 24, 492–518.

    aton, M. (2005). Is critical analysis foreign to Chinese students? In E. Manalo, & G. Wong-Toi (Eds.), Communication skills in university education: the
    international dimension (pp. 1–11). Auckland: Pearson Education.

    erkins, K. (1987). Review of the ‘test of English for international communication’. In J. C. Alderson, K. J. Krahnke, & C. W. Stansfield (Eds.), Reviews of
    English language proficiency tests (pp. 81–83). Washington, DC: TESOL.

    owers, D. E., Kim, H.-J., & Weng, V. Z. (2008). The redesigned TOEIC (listening and reading) test: relations to test-taker perceptions of proficiency in English
    (ETS Research Rep. No. RR-08-56). New Jersey: ETS; Princeton.

    ichards, J., Platt, J., & Platt, H. (1992). Longman dictionary of language teaching and applied linguistics. Harlow, UK: Longman.
    obertson, M., Line, M., Jones, S., & Thomas, S. (2000). International students, learning environments and perceptions: a case study using the Delphi

    technique. Higher Education Research & Development, 19, 89–102.
    collon, R., & Wong-Scollon, S. (1991). Topic confusion in English-Asian discourse. World Englishes, 10, 113–125.
    kehan, P., & Foster, P. (1999). The influence of task structure and processing conditions on narrative retellings. Language Learning, 49, 93–120.
    akano, Y., & Noda, A. (1993). A temporary decline of thinking ability during foreign language processing. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 24, 445–462.
    akano, Y., & Noda, A. (1995). Interlanguage dissimilarity enhances the decline of thinking ability during foreign language processing. Language Learning,

    45, 657–681.
    akano, Y. (1989). Methodological problems in cross-cultural studies of linguistic relativity. Cognition, 31, 141–162.
    en Dam, G., & Volman, M. (2004). Critical thinking as a citizenship competence: teaching strategies. Learning and Instruction, 14, 359–379.
    homm, E., & Bromme, R. (2011). It should at least seem scientific! Textual features of scientificness and their impact on lay assessments of online

    information. Science Education, 96, 187–211.
    se, C.-S., & Altarriba, J. (2008). Evidence against linguistic relativity in Chinese and English: a case study of spatial and temporal metaphors. Journal of

    Cognition and Culture, 8, 335–357.
    orster, J., & Schuring, G. (1989). Language and thought: developmental perspectives on counterfactual conditionals. South African Journal of Psychology,

    19, 34–38.
    horf, B. L. (1956). The relation of habitual thought and behavior to language. In J. B. Carroll (Ed.), Language, thought, and reality: selected writings of

    Benjamin Lee Whorf (pp. 134–159). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    olfe-Quintero, K., Inagaki, S., & Kim, H.-Y. (1998). Second language development in writing: measures of fluency, accuracy, and complexity. Technical report

    no. 17. Honolulu, H awai’i: University of Hawai’i, Second Language Teaching and Curriculum Center.

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0120

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0120

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0120

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0120

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0120

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0120

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0120

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0120

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0120

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0120

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0120

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0120

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0120

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0120

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0120

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0120

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0120

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0120

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0120

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0120

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0120

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0205

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30025-6/sbref0260

    • How might language affect critical thinking performance?
    • 1 Introduction
      1.1 Overview of the present study
      2 Study 1
      2.1 Method
      2.1.1 Participants
      2.1.2 Materials and procedure
      2.1.3 Analyses
      2.2 Results
      2.3 Discussion
      3 Study 2
      3.1 Method
      3.1.1 Participants
      3.1.2 Materials, procedure, and analysis
      3.2 Results
      3.3 Discussion
      4 General discussion
      4.1 Conclusion
      List of funder and grant number
      Conflict of interest
      Acknowledgements
      References

    Thinking Skills and Creativity 24 (2017) 140–15

    1

    Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

    Thinking Skills and Creativity

    j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : h t t p : / / w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / t s

    c

    Understanding critical thinking in Chinese sociocultural
    contexts:

    A

    case study in a Chinese college

    Lei Chen
    Shaanxi Normal University, 199, Changan Nan Road, Yanta District, Xi An, Shaan Xi, China

    a r t i c l e i n f o

    Article history:
    Received 1 November 2015
    Received in revised form 6 February 2017
    Accepted 22 February 2017
    Available online 27 February 2017

    Keywords:
    Critical thinking
    Chinese students
    Chinese indigenous philosophy
    Case study

    a b s t r a c t

    The presented research examines Chinese students’ conceptualization of critical thinking
    through a qualitative study interviewing 46 Chinese college students. This study finds that
    Chinese students tend to conceptualize critical thinking as a multi-faceted concept. Cog-
    nitive thinking skills, intellectual autonomy, and the omnipresence of positive and negative
    aspects are commonly mentioned in the students’ definitions of critical thinking (among
    other concepts). The study finds that there are unique qualities in Chinese students’ con-
    ceptualizations of critical thinking that need to be understood in Chinese sociocultural
    contexts. This project proposes ways to understand and study Chinese students not previ-
    ously considered in critical thinking literature.

    © 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

    1. Introduction

    When an English-speaking professor asks a Chinese student to think critically, does the Chinese student understand what
    he or she is required to do?

    The term critical thinking is one of several “fuzzy” constructs in education frequently used by researchers and practitioners,
    yet the definition is vague to the point of being regarded as problematic or even unnecessary by some researchers (Atkinson,
    1997; Kumaravadivelu, 2003; Vandermensbrugghe, 2004). To make the issue even more complex, critical thinking has been
    applied to classrooms and studies in other languages and cultures (Davidson, 1995; Shao, 2013;

    Z

    hang & Lambert, 2008).
    With the increasing number of Chinese students studying abroad in Western universities, there are a growing number of
    anecdotal accounts of some Western professors complaining about a lack of critical thinking among Chinese students (Clark
    & Gieve, 2006; Heng, 2016; Paton, 2005). Some professors and educators question if critical thinking can be applied to a
    Chinese educational culture which values rote-learning and respects authority and harmony (Li, Rao, & Tse, 2012; Ryan &
    Slethaug, 2010; Turner, 2006). Some scholars, however, found that Chinese students are not alien to the critical thinking
    concept and can demonstrate critical thinking when teaching is effectively conducted (Dong, Anderson, Kim, & Li, 2008;
    Durkin, 2008; Yang, 2016).

    At the same time, in several documents and public speeches released by the Chinese government Chinese educators
    announced that it is highly important to innovate Chinese education to increase the focus on critical thinking (Dong, 2015).

    Educational researchers are experimenting with various methods that could potentially improve Chinese students’ critical
    thinking (Chan, Ho, & Ku, 2011; Yang, 2016). Thus, projects contextualizing critical thinking in Chinese educational contexts
    are greatly in demand.

    E-mail address: elaine chenlei@sina.com

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2017.02.015
    1871-1871/© 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

    dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2017.02.015

    http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/18711871

    http://www.elsevier.com/locate/tsc

    http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1016/j.tsc.2017.02.015&domain=pdf

    mailto:elaine_chenlei@sina.com

    dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2017.02.015

    c

    1

    2

    3

    s
    r
    a

    1

    E
    t
    b
    t
    e

    a
    &
    a
    r
    o
    p
    I
    t

    v
    s
    a
    T

    w

    L

    p

    a
    s
    t
    r
    (
    l
    P

    t
    c
    U
    c

    a
    e
    a
    d
    c

    o
    c

    L. Chen / Thinking Skills and Creativity 24 (2017) 140–151 141

    This paper brings literature on the critical thinking concept into conversation with literature on Chinese learner’s socio-
    ultural contexts through consideration of these three research questions:

    . Can Chinese students (who have no experience studying in Western universities) provide a definition of critical thinking?

    . How do Chinese college students conceptualize critical thinking in their academic context?

    . How is such conceptualization affected by their sociocultural backgrounds?

    I draw from interviews with 46 Chinese college students to answer these questions. This paper suggests that Chinese
    tudents have unique and varied conceptualizations of critical thinking based on their sociocultural contexts. This study
    aises questions around the practice of regarding critical thinking as a predetermined and universal concept which can be
    pplied in all contexts.

    .1. The construction of critical thinking in Western literature

    The ways in which critical thinking is defined and constructed in current literature are virtually endless (Atkinson, 1997).
    ducators, philosophers, psychologists, and many others have proffered varied definitions of critical thinking. At the same
    ime, some scholars regard the construction of critical thinking as dictated by Western philosophical tradition which could
    e viewed as inherently biased against Eastern cultures. In this section, I review some widely-cited definitions of critical
    hinking and analyze how current definitions cannot be applied to educational practices which attempt to improve or
    valuate students’ critical thinking in non-Western backgrounds.

    Frequently, critical thinking is defined as a set of cognitive thinking skills including: self-directed reasoning, reflection,
    nd making decisions about what to believe (Ennis, 1996); analyzing and evaluating thinking with a view to improve it (Paul

    Elder, 2009); and problem finding as well as problem solving (Wineburg, 1998). Bloom’s Taxonomy (Bloom, 1956) includes
    hierarchical scale of skills – knowledge, comprehension, application, analysis, synthesis, and evaluation – which has been
    ecognized as connected to critical thinking (Johnston, Ford, Mitchell, & Myles, 2011). Some more recent conceptualizations
    f critical thinking are illustrated in Dwyer, Hogan, and Stewart (2014) which defines critical thinking as a metacognitive
    rocess that may purposefully increase the chances of producing a logical conclusion to an argument or solution to a problem.

    n Dwyer et al.’s framework, aside from the analytical skills and inference skills generally mentioned when defining critical
    hinking, memory, metacognition, and reflective judgment were also essential to the critical thinking concept.

    Other scholars have pointed out that some skills in Bloom’s Taxonomy, for instance, analysis and problem-solving, are
    ague and can include a number of different skills (Wegerif, Li, & Kaufman, 2015). Furthermore, what constitutes problem-
    olving skills is not universal for all people in all cultures. Cultural psychology has demonstrated that in various tasks, Asians
    pply problem-solving skills differently than Western participants (Ji, Peng, & Nisbett, 2000; Miyamoto & Wilken, 2013).
    o give one example, Ji et al. (2000) found that East Asians are more likely to incorporate contexts in problem-solving
    hile European-Americans are more likely to focus on the problem alone, ignoring contexts, when completing a Framed-

    ine Task (FLT). Cultural psychology studies remind critical thinking scholars that Asian cultures may demonstrate or value
    roblem-solving skills and higher-order thinking skills differently from Western cultures.

    Atkinson (1997), Johnston et al. (2011) and Thayer-Bacon (1992) critiqued a universal evaluative standard that encour-
    ges students to be adventurous and ask questions. Confucian teaching, for instance, believed good students to be
    elf-reflective rather than simply inquisitive. Here is a quote from The Analects of Confucius: “The Master said, I have
    alked with Hui for a whole day, and he has not made any objection to anything I said – as if he were stupid. When he
    etired, and I have examined him in private, and found him able to elucidate. Hui! – He is not stupid. “(Analects, 2:9)

    ”) According to Confucius, the exemplary student does not chal-
    enge the teacher with words. Instead, the student should reflect on himself and practice the learned philosophy with action.
    ondering the problems quietly is valued more highly than asking the teacher many questions in Confucian cultures.

    Atkinson (1997) proposed critical thinking is a social practice. Wider sociocultural contexts, for instance, demonstrate
    hat what is regarded as “good thinking” in a society influences how that culture defines critical thinking. Specifically, three
    ultural presumptions underlie the Western concepts of critical thinking: Notions of the Individual, Self-Expression, and
    sing Language to Learn. Based on these three assumptions, Atkinson further proposed that the Western definitions of
    ritical thinking may not be compatible to other cultures.

    In more recent studies of the critical thinking concept, scholars are growingly aware that critical thinking is multi-faceted
    nd multi-dimensional. For instance, Ku (2009) wrote that multiple assessments (multiple-choice response format and open
    nded format) need to be used to assess students’ critical thinking. Moore (2013) explored the definitions of critical thinking
    mong 13 university professors and found at least seven different definitions of critical thinking. Howe (2004) compared
    ifferent definitions of critical thinking from Canadian and Japanese teachers and found that more Japanese teachers defined

    ritical thinking as intellectual engagement than Canadian teachers.

    In sum, in agreement with Atkinson’s approach, I argue that critical thinking tends to be embedded in the Western ideals
    f thinking and learning as well as the minds of the practitioners. Thus, it is highly important to examine the definitions of
    ritical thinking espoused by students and teachers, as Howe (2004) and Moore (2013) demonstrated. Examination of how

    142 L. Chen / Thinking Skills and Creativity 24 (2017) 140–151

    Chinese students define critical thinking may provide significant insights for critical thinking researchers and educators of
    Chinese students around the globe.

    1.2. Critical thinking concept in Chinese educational literature

    Critical thinking in the Chinese language is a translated phrase about which Chinese scholars have yet to reach an
    agreement. Interestingly, through the scholars’ struggle some underlying cultural assumptions can be observed. The first
    translation of the word “critique” in Chinese is believed to derive from Renyuan Hu’s translation of the English version of
    Kant’s essay Critique of Pure Reason in 1935. The Chinese word for critical – (Pi Pan) – is similar to the word criticize which
    has the negative connotation of finding fault in something (Wu, 2011). As a result, Chinese scholars worried that translating
    critical thinking as (Pi Pan Xing Si Wei) may create unnecessary objections from parents, students, and education
    officials when this concept was promoted in China. Thus they recommended that instead of critical ( , Pi Pan), the Chinese
    translation of critical thinking should place more emphasis on the process of logical thinking and making decisions (Wu,
    2011). This brief history of translation reveals that Chinese scholars and researchers are concerned that the Western concept
    of critical thinking might not be easily accepted in Chinese contexts due to a variety of social, cultural, and historical reasons.

    There is also disagreement whether and how the critical thinking concept is compatible with Chinese traditional cultures,
    specifically Confucian culture. Kim (2003) suggests that traditional Chinese culture entails reflexivity, which is essential to
    critical thinking concepts. Nevertheless, a more commonly accepted judgment on the nature of Confucian teaching is from
    Graham (1989): Confucius valued learning but not thinking. Or as Kim restated, Confucius was more interested in how people
    should conduct their personal, social, and political life than the decision making process of what kind of life to live. Confucius
    did not believe that one can reach the truth through individual thinking. Unlike some Western definitions of critical thinking
    that emphasize the students’ ability to make judgments (Ennis, 1996), critical thinking in Confucian contexts seems to
    emphasize self-reflexivity, but not judgment of what kind of life to live.

    Chinese literature on the critical thinking concept demonstrates that considering Chinese contexts when defining critical
    thinking is important as this concept is foreign, borrowed, and might be incompatible with Chinese students’ sociocultural
    contexts. Thus, it is highly important to understand how Chinese students perceive this topic through empirical studies.

    1.3. Sociocultural theories of thinking and learning

    In sociocultural theories of thinking, individual efforts and sociocultural institutions and practices are constituted by and
    of each other (Rogoff, 2003). Learning is affected by the interactive, institutional, social, cultural, and historical contexts of
    the learning activity (Lave & Wenger, 1991). According to Rogoff (2003), a child’s development is affected by the norms,
    regulations, and lifestyles of the sociocultural environment in which the child lives. For instance, the concept of freedom is
    different in East and West in terms of childrearing. While for Caucasian Americans, freedom of self-expression is valued, for
    Chinese immigrants in the U.S., the ability to become self-sustaining and to make contributions to family and society is more
    important (p. 194). Sociocultural scholars also examined school cultures and minority students’ home cultures in the U.S.
    Minority students who were once regarded as incapable of effective learning were the victims of cultural mismatch between
    school and home cultures (Caffarella & Merriam, 2000). Thus, from a sociocultural approach, to understand Chinese students’
    critical thinking it is important to examine how students’ social, cultural, and educational settings shape their understanding
    and conceptualization of critical thinking.

    2. Methods

    2.1. Participants

    This project features interview data collected at a Chinese college campus during the 2012 spring semester. The research
    setting was a university in Guangzhou, a Southern metropolitan city. The participants of the study were from a variety of
    majors including Business, Literature, Education, and Engineering. Among the 46 student participants, 22 students were
    freshmen, 18 students were sophomore, 6 students were juniors; 33 were females, 13 were males. All of the participants
    were in their early twenties and came from middle to upper middle class families in cosmopolitan or suburban areas. All
    of the students were of Han ethnicity (the majority ethnicity in the People’s Republic of China). None of the students had
    studied abroad. The examination score required to enter this college is above the first-tier college entrance standard ( ),
    meaning that student motivation and the standard of learning in this university is above average in the nation. This is a large
    and comprehensive university. Most of the courses conducted in the university follow and meet national standards. In the
    English language courses I observed in the university, the faculty used government approved textbooks and urged students
    to prepare for the College English Test Band Four (CET-4). During the time this study was conducted in the university, no
    specific courses about critical thinking or other thinking skills were offered. However, most students were able to mention

    a few faculty members who used innovative methods to teach based on the faculty’s personal interests in teaching.

    As this is an exploratory case study, the participants were volunteers. Five faculty members and approximately 200
    students in the freshman, sophomore, and junior classes in the university were contacted for the purpose of participant
    recruiting. Among the 200 students, 46 students volunteered. The variety in gender and fields of study of participants

    a
    u
    t
    a
    i

    2
    w

    i
    s

    a
    i
    t
    d
    o

    i
    e
    o
    i

    2

    t
    t
    d
    m
    c
    b
    a
    i

    p
    (
    a
    d
    t
    m

    h
    c
    p
    u
    p
    f
    c

    c
    t
    d
    i
    d
    i

    L. Chen / Thinking Skills and Creativity 24 (2017) 140–151 143

    llowed me to examine a range of opinions that might exist among Chinese college students. Studies demonstrate that
    nderstanding of critical thinking may be varied among different majors (Johnston et al., 2011). Therefore, it is important
    hat students of four different areas of studies were presented. With that said, since all 46 students are from one college
    nd are volunteers, their answers may not represent all college students around China. This is a case study that proposes
    nteresting perspectives rather than providing generalized claims about how all Chinese college students think.

    .2. Interviews

    All interviews were conducted in the Chinese language and recorded, then translated and transcribed. The interviews
    ere semi-structured. The interview protocol is attached as Appendix A.

    Prior to the interviews, I spent several weeks auditing classes, eating, and talking with students in dining halls to famil-
    arize myself with the students. All interviews were conducted in classrooms and student dorm rooms to ensure that the
    tudents were comfortable talking with me.

    In the interviews, students were invited to discuss their perception of critical thinking. I used both English, critical thinking,
    nd Chinese, (Pi Pan Xing Si Wei), when I first mentioned the concept in every interview. In my first one or two
    nterviews where I explicitly used the word define ( ), the students assumed I was asking for a formal definition of critical
    hinking and replied that they did not know how to define it. When I switched to more informal wording and asked “What
    o you guess critical thinking means?” or “What do you suppose critical thinking means?” students appeared to be more
    pen to sharing their personal opinions on critical thinking.

    After the Chinese students offered their definitions of critical thinking, I asked them to offer an example of critical thinking
    n their life and in their study. I also asked the students’ opinions on how critical thinking influenced their educational
    xperiences and might influence their future careers. While the students’ definitions of critical thinking usually consisted
    f a few concise words and phrases, the specific descriptions provided by the Chinese students offered further reference to
    nterpret their definitions of critical thinking.

    .3. Analysis

    In this study the purpose of the data analysis is to explore and identify patterns of conceptualizing and discussing critical
    hinking among Chinese college students. I adopted a multi-step open-coding strategy to identify potential patterns in
    he data. In the initial analysis process, I used open-coding strategy and developed in-vivo codes that summarize student
    efinitions (Strauss, 1987). Examples of in-vivo codes include: logical thinking, problem-solving, my ideas are unique, I have
    y own judgment. In the second round of analysis, using the constant comparative method (Strauss, 1987), I categorized

    oding into a few different categories and ensured that I captured all the possible concepts of critical thinking discussed
    y the interviewed Chinese students. Then I read the coded excerpts selectively to develop themes about Chinese social
    nd cultural backgrounds which characterize the interview data. The themes, examples of in-vivo codes, and examples of
    nterview excerpts are presented in Appendix C.

    To ensure that my interpretations of the data were valid, I communicated with participants and confirmed my inter-
    retations through email. Member checking is an important technique for establishing credibility in qualitative research
    Cho & Trent, 2006; Hammersley & Atkinson, 1995; Lincoln & Guba, 1985). In this study, each participant was contacted
    t least twice during the analysis. The first time, I contacted my participants to check the categories I assigned for their
    efinitions. The second time, I contacted them to discuss my analysis of the way in which their reflections about critical
    hinking are influenced by Chinese social, cultural, historical, and educational contexts. While students agreed with most of

    y interpretations, their disagreements concerning some of the interpretations are discussed in Section 3.
    In addition, two teachers were interviewed to triangulate the data analysis (Creswell & Clark, 2007). Both Chinese teachers

    ad more than five years of college teaching experience and received graduate degrees in foreign studies and intercultural
    ommunication. These two interviews were conducted after the student interviews were coded. In these two interviews, I
    resented the teachers with Table 2 and asked if they agreed with my interpretations of the students’ definitions. This project
    sed teacher interviews to triangulate the interpretations of students’ interviews since the teachers were familiar with the
    articipants as well as the Chinese cultural contexts. This dialogic method of conducting intercultural research is adopted
    rom Tobin, Hsueh, & Karasawa (2009) who recorded pre-school scenarios in three cultures and invited local teachers to
    omment on each scenario as well as other people’s comments.

    I was aware that I enjoyed some privileges as a Chinese graduate student in a U.S. university during the time I was
    onducting the interviews. My status was respected by the students. However, there are problems being intimate with
    he participants. The fact that I was interested in the topic of critical thinking could have increased their interest and

    rawn their attention to the topic. My personal knowledge about Chinese education and Confucian culture might assist my

    nterpretation of the students’ experiences because I had an insider point of view. At the same time, such knowledge could
    elude my interpretation of the participants’ experiences and interfere with interpretation of the data. The limitations and

    mplications of my analysis are discussed in the conclusion section.

    144 L. Chen / Thinking Skills and Creativity 24 (2017) 140–151

    Table 1
    Chinese students’ abilities to provide a definition of critical thinking.

    Number of students

    Frequency

    Were able to provide one or more definitions of critical thinking and provide examples 43 93%
    Did not provide any definitions of critical thinking 2 5%
    Were able to provide a definition but were unable to support it with an example 1 2%

    Table 2
    Themes in Chinese students’ definitions of critical thinking.

    Themes Codes Frequency

    Cognitive thinking skills Quality reasoning and sound judgment; 36 students
    Logical thinking; 75%
    Problem-solving;
    Evidence based thinking;
    Deliberate and controlled thinking strategies.

    Intellectual autonomy Having their own ideas; 27 students
    Unique ideas; 56%
    Freedom to make own judgment;
    Conduct own research;
    Freedom to use imagination.

    Omnipresence of the opposite point of view Positive and negative; 11 students
    Pros and cons. 23%

    Multiple perspectives (More than two, and
    aware of conflicts)

    Thinking in a variety of perspectives; 8 students
    Torn between different perspectives. 17%

    Revolutionary thoughts in art, science, and
    other fields.

    Revolutionary ideas; 7 students
    People who bring revolutions to their fields. 15%

    Knowledge and skills outside the scope of
    standard examination

    The knowledge and skills that are
    useful in real life, but cannot be tested.

    1 student
    2%

    3. Results

    3.1. The Chinese college students’ abilities to define critical thinking

    The majority of the students in this project did not have difficulty providing definitions of critical thinking and were
    able to provide examples from their own experiences to illustrate critical thinking. However, in two interviews, students
    explicitly told me that they were not familiar with the critical thinking concept and refused to offer any definitions of
    critical thinking. Both of the students were freshmen and believed they had never heard about the critical thinking concept
    in their previous educational experiences. In both interviews, I asked the students why they would volunteer if they had
    no idea what the critical thinking concept meant. The students expressed that they were interested to learn the concept
    of critical thinking and would like me to define the concept for them. I offered a few definitions of critical thinking such
    as higher-order thinking, intellectual autonomy, argumentation, etc. Both students understood the explanations and were
    able to give examples of such based on their own understanding. However, they refused to tell me which definitions most
    aligned with their conceptions of critical thinking. One Chinese student provided a definition but could not provide any
    supporting examples. After I repeatedly prompted her to try, she said she wasn’t sure about the definition and would rather
    that I provided a definition and an example for her. The students’ abilities to provide a definition of critical thinking is listed
    in Table 1.

    The Chinese students’ ability to define critical thinking demonstrates that critical thinking is not alien to Chinese college
    students. Although two students could not define critical thinking, they did understand the concepts related to critical
    thinking. Note again that all the participants were volunteers and the percentage provided in this study might not represent
    the ability to define critical thinking by all Chinese college students.

    3.2. The multiplicity of students’ definitions

    In the interviews, most students mentioned several different themes. For instance, a student said in her interview, “First
    of all, I have my own judgment, and also, I have a reason to support my own judgment.” According to my analysis, two themes
    – cognitive thinking skills and intellectual autonomy – were mentioned in her short, one sentence answer. Sometimes, a

    student would define critical thinking in one way when asked to provide a definition and another way when giving an
    example of critical thinking or discussing real life cases in which critical thinking is important. During the member checking
    process, I checked with students if all definitions in their answers were valid conceptualizations of critical thinking. I also
    verified that there were no other ways to define critical thinking other than what they had mentioned.


    i
    w

    3

    d
    c
    s
    c
    s
    s
    fi
    c

    i
    t
    c

    3

    t
    c
    d
    a
    a
    c

    (
    d
    s
    o
    i
    t

    c
    c
    n
    t
    s

    3

    w
    s
    a
    t
    f
    K

    p
    b
    o
    a
    c
    c
    t
    i

    L. Chen / Thinking Skills and Creativity 24 (2017) 140–151 145

    In one interview, the student specified that her conceptualization was the only way to define critical thinking. She stated
    Only great writers have critical thinking.” When I asked her if a college student could have critical thinking skills, she noted
    n very rare cases, when a college student produced publishable work, that would be an example of critical thinking. She

    as the only student that conceptualized critical thinking with one absolute definition.

    .3. The popular themes among Chinese students’ definitions of critical thinking

    The themes and frequency of Chinese students’ definitions are presented in Table 2. Note that most of the Chinese stu-
    ents mentioned more than one theme in the interviews so the total frequency is greater than 100%. The most popular
    onceptualization is critical thinking as a cognitive skill. 75% of the students mentioned some cognitive terms such as rea-
    oning, evidence, or backing up claims in their definitions of critical thinking. 27 students conceptualized critical thinking
    oncept as intellectual autonomy, making it the second most commonly mentioned definition among the Chinese college
    tudents in this project. 22 students mentioned the omnipresence of the opposite point of view, and 10 students mentioned
    ome concept of thinking with multiple perspectives. 7 students mentioned revolutionary thoughts in art, science, and other
    elds as critical thinking, and one student mentioned knowledge and skills that are useful in life, but cannot be tested in schools as
    ritical thinking. I will explain in the following sections each concept used to characterize the Chinese students’ definitions.

    As this is a case study conducted with volunteers, the various ways critical thinking was defined by the Chinese students
    n this project does not represent how critical thinking would be defined by all Chinese students across the nation. However,
    he analysis of the unique features of Chinese students’ definitions of critical thinking may indicate how Chinese culture
    ould influence Chinese students’ understanding of critical thinking.

    .3.1. Cognitive thinking skills
    Cognitive thinking skills, including logical thinking, problem-solving, and other higher-order thinking strategies, were men-

    ioned by 36 students in the interviews making it the most commonly mentioned way of conceptualizing the critical thinking
    oncept. The Chinese students offered a variety of examples demonstrating the use of some cognitive thinking skills in aca-
    emic settings as critical thinking. For instance, five students regarded logical thinking and problem-solving in math studies
    s examples of critical thinking. Six students described decision making using certain thinking strategies in extra curriculum
    ctivities, student groups, internship, selecting their majors, and various other contexts to illustrate their understanding of
    ritical thinking.

    Critical thinking defined as cognitive thinking skills is consistent with the definition provided by many educational scholars
    Ennis, 1996; Paul & Elder, 2009). This similarity demonstrates that despite the Confucian cultural influence which values
    ifferent methods of reasoning compared to Western reasoning, the concept of logical reasoning is not alien to Chinese college
    tudents. Chinese college students do not live in a bubble of Chinese traditional culture. They understand the importance
    f logical thinking, problem-solving, and reasoning based upon evidence, at least theoretically. Similar results have been found
    n other studies; in Jones (2004), Chinese students taking an economic class in Australia demonstrated an understanding of
    he faculty’s critical thinking requirements similar to the Australian students.

    However, there is disagreement among Chinese students whether cognitive thinking skills can capture the critical thinking
    oncept in this project. While in member checking, most students confirmed that the thinking skills they mentioned are
    ritical thinking; however, one student explicitly said, “No, reasoning is like thinking according to certain logic, there’s
    othing critical in that.” When I showed this case to the Chinese teachers, both teachers said they were surprised but found
    he student’s position understandable. This example demonstrates that defining critical thinking as higher order thinking
    kills is not agreed upon by every Chinese student in this project.

    .3.2. Intellectual autonomy
    In this project, 27 students spoke of their conceptions of critical thinking as having their own ideas. They provided examples

    hen their opinions disagreed with their parents or teachers to emphasize the originality of their own ideas. This is the
    econd most common definition that Chinese students provided in this project after cognitive thinking skills. Intellectual
    utonomy is essential in Kantian philosophy, which proposes that man should be capable of using his own understanding in
    ruth seeking without the guidance of another person (Kant, 1996). Students’ confidence to disagree with authority, to want
    reedom, and to believe in their own ability to make judgments is a demonstration of the intellectual autonomy valued by
    ant and many other Western educators (Facione, Sánchez, Facione, & Gainen, 1995; Howe, 2004; Paul & Elder, 2009).

    Intellectual autonomy shares some similarities and differences with the critical thinking disposition (Ennis, 1996) –
    eople’s willingness to seek “scientific” conceptualizations of a subject or problem. A critical thinking disposition includes
    eing “broad and adventurous,” the desire to “clarify and seek understanding” (p.169), and to “seek alternatives and be
    pen to them” (p. 165). In my analysis, there are similarities between critical thinking as a disposition and the intellectual
    utonomy emphasized in Chinese students’ definitions of critical thinking. Both concepts emphasize students’ willingness,

    ourage, and motivation to question preexisting assumptions. However, there are differences in the emphasis of the two
    oncepts. Whereas in Chinese students’ definitions the emphasis lays in students’ ownership of their ideas, in the critical
    hinking disposition the emphasis is on using scientific methods to seek truth. The Chinese students emphasized that their
    deas of critical thinking are set against an authority’s perspective, which can be scientific or not.

    146 L. Chen / Thinking Skills and Creativity 24 (2017) 140–151

    Following is an excerpt from a female student, Jin. Jin was the first student who volunteered to be interviewed and very
    outspoken. Throughout the interviews, she expressed her personal opinions on a few different issues, including the Chinese
    educational system, the management of her university, and the economic development of her hometown. Jin defined critical
    thinking as follows:

    Author: What do you think the term critical thinking means to your understanding?

    Jin: The first thing [in defining critical thinking] is critical, to critique or criticize. Critique, to me, means I have my own
    opinions. I have my own opinions towards a particular topic, and my ideas are different from other people, from my
    teachers and my parents. And then, I have evidence. I have a reason to support my opinions. That’s it pretty much.
    [05/15/2012]

    In Jin’s interview, she emphasized that having her own opinions different from the authority is crucial to the critical thinking
    concept, and then she had to support her own idea with evidence, which is a demonstration that she valued higher order logical
    thinking as part of critical thinking at the same time. When I asked Jin to give an example of what she meant by her own
    ideas, she gave an example in which she disagreed with the school policy of mandatory internship at the end of senior year.

    Jin: . . .You know that, when the college students do the internship, they [college students] are treated as cheap labor.
    A lot of them [students] are not doing major-related work. They [students] are working for better resume, or school
    credit. I think this is not worth it. [05/15/2012]

    Jin emphasized the words “I,” “my,” and “I think” with intonation in the interview. Those words demonstrate the freedom
    of thought that she valued as critical thinking. Her opinions about mandatory internship were against the opinion of the
    authority.

    Another female student, Hui, expressed similar ideas in her interview.

    Author: . . .. . .What do you suppose critical thinking means?

    Hui: Critical thinking emphasizes having opinion of my own. The teacher does not put restrictions on me. I have the
    freedom to make my own judgment. [05/28/2012]

    In Hui’s answer, she regarded her teacher as the authority and critical thinking as the freedom to make her own judgment
    without teacher or authority interference, scientifically or not. Hui later gave an example of answering open-ended questions
    in social studies classes as an example of critical thinking. In this sense, unlike Jin whose concept of intellectual freedom is
    against authority, Hui’s freedom was granted by authority.

    In the following example, Shipeng, a male student, discusses his idea of critical thinking. He expressed suspicion of the
    teachers’ professional competence to illustrate his conceptualization of critical thinking, although from his point of view,
    this spirit of suspicion might not help him in his studies.

    Author: What do you think critical thinking means?

    Shipeng: Critical thinking is, when your teacher taught something, you don’t take things for granted, but think about
    it· · ·Sometimes it may interfere with your study.
    Author: Why is that?

    Shipeng: Sometimes, I have doubt in what the teacher is talking about in class, I will not listen to the rest of it; I will ignore
    it. I would rather search on the Internet for it and learn the stuff myself. Sometimes, maybe the teacher was right, (in that
    case) I would miss it (what the teacher has to say). . .Those things may be tested. So to some extent critical thinking
    may interfere with my study. But not absolutely. If I can learn everything after the class, then that’s fine. [05/30/2012]

    Shipeng questioned his teachers in class and developed his own method to test his hypothesis. Instead of regarding critical
    thinking as the desire to seek a scientific conceptualization of a problem (Ennis, 1996), Shipeng emphasized discovering the
    conceptualization of a problem in his own way. In the interview, it seems that Shipeng believed his pursuit of critical thinking
    might be punished with low scores on tests which value reproduction of the detailed knowledge the teacher imparted rather
    than the process of finding out information (Dong, 2015).

    During member checking, interviewed students reiterated that they emphasized “having their own ideas” as critical
    thinking, while some students reminded me that they defined critical thinking as a multi-faceted concept and “being able
    to offer evidence for their own ideas” is also important.

    In the interviews with Chinese teachers, one commented on Jin’s definition, “this (students’ ownership of ideas) can
    be critical thinking, but this is too simple. It is not enough for students to have their own opinions to demonstrate critical
    thinking. The students’ opinions need to be properly defended, reasoned, and examined” (Interview, 09/02/2012). After
    some discussion, we agreed that it was not for the researcher or the teachers to judge the students’ definitions, but to seek

    to understand them. Thus, the Chinese students’ understanding of critical thinking that emphasizes having their own ideas
    was theorized by this project as intellectual autonomy.

    L. Chen / Thinking Skills and Creativity 24 (2017) 140–151 147

    3.3.3. The omnipresence of opposite point of views
    19 (40%) Chinese students in the interviews defined critical thinking as consideration of diverse aspects of an issue when

    making a judgment. Those students explicitly mentioned “considering multiple aspects of an issue when making a judgment”
    or “being aware that there is (a) con coming with every pro.”

    On the surface, such definitions are in agreement with some Western scholars of critical thinking. Blodgett-McDeavitt
    (1995) as cited in Johnston et al. (2011) defines critical thinking as “thinking about other alternatives” and “looking at the
    bigger scheme of things” (p. 38). However, in my interpretation there is something special about the Chinese version of
    “thinking about other alternatives”. Among the 19 students who mentioned considering multiple perspectives/angles, 11 (23%
    of overall sample) specifically indicated or offered examples of “positive and negative aspects” ( ) when defining
    critical thinking. This description of pros and cons is influenced by the Chinese indigenous philosophy of dialectics in my
    analysis.

    For example, Pingmei described the process of making a decision about whether to join an extracurricular student group
    as an example of critical thinking. The positive side of joining a student group is practicing social skills, and the negative side
    is wasting time that should be spent on study. I asked her if, with the co-existence of positive and negative sides, she could
    make up her mind. She said the decision could not be made because there was always cons. Eventually, she said the wise
    decision could only be made a year later when she became more experienced with college life. Thus, in my analysis, critical
    thinking as omnipresence of pros and cons helps the students to realize their own limitations while not necessarily assisting
    in decision making.

    Shan gave another example where she made a decision based on her need, while critical thinking as the omnipresence of
    pros and cons helped her realize her limitations:

    Shan: I think critical thinking is a way of thinking, you don’t always think about things in positive ways, there is
    negative side in everything too· · ·For example, people around me talk about going to graduate school after college.
    With critical thinking, I think about the negative things about it, I won’t be able to earn income for long.

    Author: So, how are you going to make the decision based on the pros and cons?

    Shan: Actually, I am preparing to apply to graduate school· · ·I think in college, you only learned some surface infor-
    mation about a particular field. I want to learn more in-depth things.

    Author: So you made a decision against your critical thinking?

    Shan: That’s right. I made a decision according to my need. Critical thinking helps me be aware of the negative side of my
    decision. [05/28/2012, The emphasis is the author’s.]

    In this example, Shan tried to make a decision for herself instead of following the people around her. This can be understood
    as intellectual autonomy. Shan also sees critical thinking as an awareness that she needs to think about both the pros and
    cons of going to graduate school. However, after weighing the pros and cons, her decision was made based on other factors.

    In another example, Meiru explicitly stated that the term she used for defining critical thinking, the theory of two sides
    ( ), is adopted from her Chinese high school philosophy curriculum.

    Author: What do you think critical thinking means?

    Fig. 1. Yin-Yang symbol.

    148 L. Chen / Thinking Skills and Creativity 24 (2017) 140–151

    Meiru: It is like in the politics and philosophy class. . .[We learned that] when thinking about something, you cannot
    just look at it from one side, you have to think about it according to the theory of two sides ( ). For one single
    matter, it is possible that we would see it from different positions, and perspectives. [05/23/2012]

    Meiru adopted a key concept from her Chinese high school philosophy curriculum to illustrate critical thinking. The key
    concept was “considering a problem by dividing it into two ( )” (Philosophical Textbook, 2010).

    According to Peng, Spencer-Rodgers, and Nian (2006), the Chinese high school philosophy curriculum covers Chinese
    Indigenous Philosophy, which was widely misattributed and politically propagated under the cover of Marxism. Meiru’s
    understanding of critical thinking was a reflection of Chinese Indigenous Philosophy, although she remembered it as part of
    philosophy class.

    Nisbett (2003) described the Chinese dialectic (Chinese Indigenous Philosophy) and its difference with Western philos-
    ophy in the following passage:

    [In Western philosophy], thesis is followed by antithesis, which is resolved by synthesis, and which is “aggressive”
    in the sense that the ultimate goal of reasoning is to resolve contradiction. The Chinese dialectic instead uses contra-
    diction to understand relations among objects or events. . . In the Chinese intellectual tradition there is no necessary
    incompatibility between the belief that A is the case and the belief that not-A is the case. . .A can actually imply that
    not-A is also the case, or at any rate soon will be the case. (¨ )̈ . . .It is the Middle Way that is the goal of reasoning.”

    Chinese Indigenous Philosophy was adopted by some Chinese students to illustrate their conceptualization of critical
    thinking in this project. Chinese Indigenous Philosophy states that in life, matter, and the world, there are always contradic-
    tions. In everything black, there is something white. In everything white, there is something black, as illustrated by Yin-Yang
    symbol. The balanced harmony is emphasized rather than contradictions. Chinese Indigenous Philosophy has been cited by
    many cultural psychology studies to illustrate and understand the thinking process of Chinese lay people (Boucher, Peng,
    Shi, & Wang, 2009; Needham, 1954; Peng et al., 2006) (Fig. 1).

    There is a similar concept about contradictions in Western philosophy. For instance, Janusian thinking, named for the
    Roman god Janus who has two faces, each looking at the opposite direction. However, as Nisbett (2003) described, the
    Western idea of contradiction emphasizes the conflict and reasoning process necessary to resolve the contradiction. Janusian
    thinking is regarded as a source of creativity by many Western scholars (Rothenberg, 1971). This is quite different from
    Chinese Indigenous Philosophy’s emphasis on the harmonious state of two opposites. Chinese Indigenous Philosophy has
    been said to have a negative influence on creativity (Heng, 2016) – since there are always going to be cons, it is better to
    make peace rather than resolve them. This may explain why interviewed students in this study did not find Indigenous
    philosophy to be very useful in decision making.

    It is important to point out that not all the interviewed students thought about critical thinking in the context of Chinese
    Indigenous Philosophy. Eight noted that they understood the complexities and contradictions between the “positive” and
    “negative” sides. Many of them mentioned “one question leads to another” and “there is no answer” in defining critical
    thinking. These answers demonstrate an effort to explore and resolve the contradiction, similar to the Western ideas of
    critical thinking such as “to be broad and adventurous,” to “clarify and seek understanding” (Ennis, 1996, p. 169), and to
    “seek alternatives and be open to them” (p. 165).

    In member checking, the Chinese students and both Chinese teachers confirmed that critical thinking can be defined
    as considering a problem by dividing it into two ( ), the principle learned from philosophy classes and Chinese
    Indigenous Philosophy.

    4. Conclusion

    Scholars who write about critical thinking concepts rarely consider the voices of students, especially Chinese students, and
    even those scholars that do focus on Chinese students’ learning habits rarely problematize the concept of critical thinking.
    This project fills this gap and hopes to make Chinese student voices available to researchers. The case study of 46 Chinese
    students increases our knowledge of Chinese students’ understanding of the critical thinking concept and how it may be
    influenced by sociocultural contexts. This study raises questions around the practice of regarding critical thinking as a
    predetermined and universal concept that can be applied in all contexts.

    The students in this project tend to mention several ways to conceptualize critical thinking simultaneously in one inter-
    view. Very few students were unable to define critical thinking or provide suitable definitions in the interviews. Furthermore,
    those students were interested to learn about the concept of critical thinking and were able to understand different concepts
    related to critical thinking such as evidence-based reasoning and logical thinking.

    When asked to define critical thinking, cognitive thinking skills, intellectual autonomy and the omnipresence of positive and
    negative sides were commonly mentioned by the interviewed students. The emphasis on ownership of ideas against authority
    is characterized as intellectual autonomy in this project. The students’ understanding of pros and cons and the significance of

    harmony was also discussed. Intellectual autonomy and the omnipresence of positive and negative sides reflect the influences
    of Chinese Indigenous Philosophy and the Western concept of individualism, among many other influences.

    This study challenges the stereotype that Chinese students tend to be obedient, which has been refuted in other studies
    such as Grimshaw (2007), an ethnographic study in a university in China. Grimshaw found some Chinese college students

    r
    s
    t
    i
    a

    s
    t
    s
    m
    b
    e

    d
    a
    a
    m
    c
    (

    c
    p
    c
    c

    s
    s
    T
    c

    A

    L
    A










    A

    L. Chen / Thinking Skills and Creativity 24 (2017) 140–151 149

    ead books on their own instead of paying attention to lectures when they disliked the professor’s teaching. The result of this
    tudy echoes previous research conducted on Japanese teachers’ conceptualization of critical thinking (Howe, 2004); critical
    hinking defined as individualistic thinking was highly valued in East Asian contexts. Further studies are needed to examine
    n more detail how Asian social and cultural contexts affect/promote students’ intellectual autonomy. Furthermore, studies
    re needed to analyze how to meet the need of teachers and students to express their authentic opinions in the classroom.

    Another feature of the Chinese student definitions of critical thinking is the omnipresence of opposite points of views. It
    eems that, influenced by Chinese culture and the high school philosophical curriculum, Chinese students have a tendency
    o understand that problems and issues can be analyzed in terms of positive and negative sides. However, some Chinese
    tudents appeared to lack the understanding of the complexities involved in such dichotomy, leading to reluctance in decision
    aking since there would always be some negative result. This is discussed as a limitation of Chinese Indigenous Philosophy

    y some researchers (Dong, 2015). Further studies are needed to explore how to promote thinking beyond merely the
    xistence of positive and negative sides when teaching Chinese students.

    This finding highlights the complexities and conflicts in defining critical thinking. Some concepts were regarded as a
    emonstration of critical thinking for some students, but a lack of it for others. Some students regarded logical reasoning
    s critical thinking while others dismissed the idea. It is possible that a Chinese student stating their personal opinions on

    controversial topic may be regarded as a demonstration of critical thinking in the student’s perspective. However, faculty
    ay not consider this statement critical thinking because it lacks sufficient evidence and reasoning. The ambiguity and

    omplexities in defining critical thinking often found by scholars can also be found in the opinions of students and teachers
    Kumaravadivelu, 2003; Vandermensbrugghe, 2004).

    This project is aware that overemphasizing the differences between Chinese and Western students with regards to
    ritical thinking may contribute to stereotyping Chinese students, especially in international education. There are important
    arallels in how critical thinking is defined by Chinese students and the Western definitions of critical thinking. The most
    ommonly mentioned concept in all the interviews was critical thinking as cognitive thinking skills. This is similar to how
    ritical thinking is defined by Western scholars (Bloom, 1956; Ennis, 1996; Paul & Elder, 2009).

    There are limitations in this study. The sample in this project is too small to make generalized claims about how Chinese
    tudents think across China. The project was conducted by a single author instead of a team. Further study with a larger data
    et is needed to examine if there are other prominent patterns in the definitions of critical thinking among Chinese students.
    his project is a teaser proposing methods to study critical thinking from student perspectives rather than establishing
    ritical thinking as a standardized and pre-determined concept.

    cknowledgement

    I would also like to show my gratitude to my colleagues from University of Delaware, including Eugene Matusov, Anselmo
    ima, Katherine von Duyke, and Rosalie Rolón Dow, for their assistance, guidance and comments.

    ppendix A. Interview protocol.

    What is your major of study?
    How far are you in your study?
    Have you heard the term “critical thinking” before?
    Have you taken any courses concerning critical thinking or other thinking skills?
    What do you think “critical thinking” means?
    Can you give an example of “critical thinking”?
    Can you give an example when you applied “critical thinking” in your work?
    Do you think “critical thinking” is useful for your study? How?
    Can you give an example when your teacher nurtured your “critical thinking”?
    Do you think critical thinking might be useful for your future career?

    ppendix B. Chinese transcript of the selected interview.

    150 L. Chen / Thinking Skills and Creativity 24 (2017) 140–151

    Appendix C. Example of codes and interview transcripts.

    Themes Examples of Students’ definitions
    (in-vivo codes)

    Examples of critical thinking given by the students in the
    interviews

    Frequency

    Cognitive thinking
    skills

    Quality reasoning and sound judgment
    Logical thinking
    Problem-solving
    Evidence based thinking
    Deliberate and controlled thinking
    strategies
    Argumentative writing

    “Try to look at an issue from different perspectives so you
    can be more objective.”
    “Solving a math problem with your own method.”
    “I have a reason to support my opinion.”
    “When I make a decision, it is not just according to my
    feeling, but I write it down, think (it) over carefully, and
    evaluate different choices.”
    “To solve a math problem, I ask myself, what do I need to
    know to answer this question? It’s a controlled process,
    step by step.”
    “In writing class, the teacher tried to make us argue with
    him about a variety of issues, like, is using Weibo (Chinese
    Twitter) a positive or negative thing for a college student?”
    “I think my writing teacher really wants us to develop
    critical thinking and he looks for that when he evaluates
    students’ work.”
    “Critical thinking skill is used a lot in debate competition.”

    36 students.
    75%

    Intellectual autonomy Having my own ideas.
    My ideas are unique.
    I have the freedom to make my own
    judgment.
    I have an idea that is different than the
    teachers or the parents.
    I do my own research.
    The students have freedom to use their
    imagination. There is no suppressing
    the students.

    “I think mandatory internship is hurtful to students’
    development.”
    “I want to apply to graduate school but my parents want
    me to find a job. I will continue with my own plan despite
    their disagreement.”
    “If I have doubts about what the teacher is talking about in
    class, I would stop listening to her and do my own
    research.”
    “When the teacher introduces us to something, especially
    some opinions, I should not just accept it, but also think
    about it.”

    27 students.
    56%

    Omnipresence of the
    opposite point of
    view

    Not always thinking something as
    positive, but also seeing the negative
    side in it.
    You can agree with something, and
    disagree with it at the same time. Not
    one-sided opinion.
    There is something good and
    something bad about everything.
    Critique the things that are bad, and
    continue with the things that are good.
    Evaluate the pros and cons

    “For instance, in the history class, whenever you are asked
    to evaluate something, you always say the positive side
    and the negative side of the issue. The positive side of the
    revolution is that it leads the country to a better society;
    the bad side is many people are sacrificed in the wars.”
    “I can only give an example of my action that lacks critical
    thinking. During the first semester in freshman year, I
    planned to join a student group. I thought about what kind
    of benefits those student groups can offer me. I didn’t
    really pay attention to the disadvantages of joining them.”

    11 students.
    23%

    Multiple perspectives
    (More than two, and
    aware of the
    conflicts)

    Thinking in a variety of perspectives.
    Torn between different perspectives

    “Some movies or novels made me think critically,
    like· · ·Fight Club?”
    “In some novels, there are no bad people but there’s no
    real solution to the problem in the novel. I am torn
    between different perspectives.”
    “Sometimes, the teacher in social studies may present us
    with a social problem that just inspires us to think, like the
    vast difference between cities and countryside in China,
    what caused the problem, what is the solution, one
    question leads to another? No easy answer.”

    8 students.
    17%

    Revolutionary thoughts
    in art, science and
    other fields

    Writers.
    Scientists.
    Philosophers.
    People who revolutionize their fields.

    “Only great writers have critical thinking, like Lu Xun. He
    critiqued the society of his time with deep insight.”
    “I think what Copernicus did, that was an example of
    critical thinking.”
    “Critical thinking makes me think of philosophers like
    Kant, Descartes, and Marx.”

    7 students
    15%

    Self-explorative study I did my own research “Sometimes, we will be asked to do a project, a research
    project on a certain topic. There’s no definite answer. We
    just looked for different sources. It is true inquiry.”

    2 students.
    4%

    Knowledge and skills
    outside the scope of
    standard

    The knowledge and skills that are
    useful in real life, but cannot be tested.

    “I didn’t come to college to pass the exams. University is
    not just about studying, or exams. The stuff that is useful in
    real life but cannot be tested, the extra stuff, that is critical

    1 students
    2%

    examination thinking.”

    R

    A
    B
    B

    C
    C
    C
    C

    C

    D

    D
    D

    D

    D
    E
    F
    G
    G

    H
    H

    H

    J

    J
    J

    K
    K
    K

    K
    L
    L

    L
    M

    M
    N
    N
    P

    P
    P

    R
    R
    R

    S

    S
    T
    T
    T

    V

    W
    W

    W
    Y

    Z

    L. Chen / Thinking Skills and Creativity 24 (2017) 140–151 151

    eferences

    tkinson, D. (1997). A critical approach to critical thinking in TESOL. TESOL Quarterly, 31(1), 71–94.
    loom, B. S. (1956). Taxonomy of educational objectives. Vol. 1: Cognitive domain. pp. 20–24. New York: McKay.
    oucher, H. C., Peng, K., Shi, J., & Wang, L. (2009). Culture and implicit self-esteem: Chinese are good and bad at the same time. Journal of Cross-Cultural

    Psychology, 40(1), 24–45.
    affarella, R., & Merriam, S. B. (2000). Linking the individual learner to the context of adult learning. Handbook of Adult and Continuing Education, 55–70.
    han, N., Ho, I. T., & Ku, K. Y. (2011). Epistemic beliefs and critical thinking of Chinese students. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(1), 67–77.
    ho, J., & Trent, A. (2006). Validity in qualitative research revisited. Qualitative Research, 6(3), 319–340.
    lark, R., & Gieve, S. N. (2006). On the discursive construction of ‘the Chinese learner’. Language Culture and Curriculum, 19(1), 54–73.

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/07908310608668754
    reswell, J. W., & Clark, V. L. P. (2007). Designing and conducting mixed methods research. California: Sage Publications, Inc.
    avidson, B. W. (1995). Critical thinking education faces the challenge of Japan. Inquiry: Critical Thinking Across the Disciplines, 14(3), 41–53.
    ong, T., Anderson, R. C., Kim, I., & Li, Y. (2008). Collaborative reasoning in China and korea. Reading Research Quarterly, 43(4), 400–424.
    ong, Y. (2015). Critical thinking education with Chinese characteristics. In M. Davies, & R. Barnett (Eds.), The Palgrave handbook of critical thinking in

    higher education (pp. 351–368). New York: Springer.
    urkin, K. (2008). The adaptation of East Asian masters students to western norms of critical thinking and argumentation in the UK. Intercultural

    Education, 19(1), 15–27.
    wyer, C. P., Hogan, M. J., & Stewart, I. (2014). An integrated critical thinking framework for the 21 st century. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 12, 43–52.
    nnis, R. H. (1996). Critical thinking dispositions: Their nature and assessability. Informal Logic, 18(2), 165–182.
    acione, P. A., Sánchez, C. A., Facione, N. C., & Gainen, J. (1995). The disposition toward critical thinking. The Journal of General Education, 44(1), 1–25.
    raham, A. C. (1989). Disputers of the Tao: Philosophical argument in ancient China. La Salle: Open Court.
    rimshaw, T. (2007). Problematizing the construct of ‘the Chinese learner’: insights from ethnographic research. Educational Studies, 33(3), 299–311.

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03055690701425643
    ammersley, M., & Atkinson, P. (1995). Ethnography: Practices and principles. New York: Routledge.
    eng, T. T. (2016). Different is not deficient: Contradicting stereotypes of Chinese international students in US higher education. Studies in Higher

    Education, 1–15.
    owe, E. R. (2004). Canadian and Japanese teachers’ conceptions of critical thinking: A comparative study. Teachers and Teaching, 10(5), 505–525.

    i, L., Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (2000). Culture, control, and perception of relationships in the environment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
    78(5), 943.

    ohnston, B., Ford, P., Mitchell, R., & Myles, F. (2011). Developing student criticality in higher education: Undergraduate learning in the arts and social sciences.
    London: Bloomsbury Publishing.

    ones, A. (2004). Teaching critical thinking: An investigation of a task in introductory macroeconomics. Higher Education Research & Development, 23(2),
    167–181.

    ant, E. (1785/1996). The metaphysics of morals: Cambridge texts in the history of philosophy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    im, H. (2003). Critical thinking, learning and confucius: A positive assessment. Journal of Philosophy of Education, 37(1), 71–87.
    u, K. Y. (2009). Assessing students’ critical thinking performance: Urging for measurements using multi-response format. Thinking Skills and Creativity,

    4(1), 70–76.
    umaravadivelu, B. (2003). Problematizing cultural stereotypes in TESOL. TESOL Quarterly, 37(4), 709–719.
    ave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    i, H., Rao, N., & Tse, S. K. (2012). Adapting western pedagogies for Chinese literacy instruction: Case studies of Hong Kong, Shenzhen, and Singapore

    preschools. Early Education & Development, 23(4), 603–621.
    incoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
    iyamoto, Y., & Wilken, B. (2013). Cultural differences and their mechanisms. In D. Reisberg (Ed.), Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Psychology (pp. 970–985).

    Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    oore, T. (2013). Critical thinking: Seven definitions in search of a concept. Studies in Higher Education, 38(4), 506–522.
    eedham, J. (1954). . Science and civilization in China (Vol. I) Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    isbett, R. E. (2003). The geography of thought: How Asians and Westerners think differently and why. New York: Free Press.
    aton, M. (2005). Is critical analysis foreign to Chinese students. In E. Manalo, & G. Wong-Toi (Eds.), Communication skills in university education: The

    international dimension (pp. 1–11). Auckland, New Zealand: Person Education New Zealand.
    aul, R., & Elder, L. (2009). Critical thinking: Ethical reasoning and fairminded thinking, part I. Journal of Developmental Education, 33(1), 36.
    eng, K., Spencer-Rodgers, J., & Nian, Z. (2006). Naïve dialecticism and the Tao of Chinese thought. In U. Kim, K. Yang, & G. Huang (Eds.), Indigenous and

    cultural psychology: Understanding people in contexts (pp. 247–262). New York: Springer.
    ogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    othenberg, A. (1971). The process of Janusian thinking in creativity. Archives of General Psychiatry, 24(3), 195–205.
    yan, J., & Slethaug, G. (Eds.). (2010). International education and the Chinese learner (Vol. 1). Hongkong: Hong Kong University Press.

    hao, W. (2013). . A brief review of the core value of nurturing critical
    thinking and self-directed knowledge construction in current higher education. University Education, 2013, 14.

    trauss, A. L. (1987). Qualitative analysis for social scientists. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    hayer-Bacon, B. (1992). Is modern critical thinking theory sexist? Inquiry: Critical Thinking Across the Disciplines, 10(1), 3–7.
    obin, J., Hsueh, Y., & Karasawa, M. (2009). Preschool in three cultures revisited: China, Japan, and the United States. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
    urner, Y. (2006). Students from mainland china and critical thinking in postgraduate business and management degrees: Teasing out tensions of culture,

    style and substance. International Journal of Management Education, 5(1), 3–11.
    andermensbrugghe, J. (2004). The unbearable vagueness of critical thinking in the context of the Anglo-Saxonisation of education. International

    Education Journal, 5(3), 417–422.
    egerif, R., Li, L., & Kaufman, J. C. (2015). The Routledge international handbook of research on teaching thinking. London: Routledge.
    ineburg, S. (1998). Reading Abraham Lincoln: An expert/expert study in the interpretation of historical texts. Cognitive Science, 22(3), 319–346.

    u H. (2011). (Critical thinking: study of the semantics) Journal of Yanan University (Social Science), 33(1), 5–17.
    ang, Y. (2016). Lessons learnt from contextualising a UK teaching thinking program in a conventional Chinese classroom. Thinking Skills and Creativity,

    19, 198–209.
    hang, H., & Lambert, V. (2008). Critical thinking dispositions and learning styles of baccalaureate nursing students from china. Nursing & Health Sciences,

    10(3), 175–181.

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0005

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0030

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0030

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0030

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0030

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0030

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0030

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0030

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0030

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0030

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0030

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0030

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0030

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0030

    dx.doi.org/10.1080/07908310608668754

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0040

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0050

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0055

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0060

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0080

    dx.doi.org/10.1080/03055690701425643

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0090

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0095

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0100

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0125

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0130

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0135

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0165

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0175

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0180

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0195

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0200

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0235

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0240

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(17)30054-8/sbref0255

    • Understanding critical thinking in Chinese sociocultural contexts: A case study in a Chinese college
    • 1 Introduction
      1.1 The construction of critical thinking in Western literature
      1.2 Critical thinking concept in Chinese educational literature
      1.3 Sociocultural theories of thinking and learning
      2 Methods
      2.1 Participants
      2.2 Interviews
      2.3 Analysis
      3 Results
      3.1 The Chinese college students’ abilities to define critical thinking
      3.2 The multiplicity of students’ definitions
      3.3 The popular themes among Chinese students’ definitions of critical thinking
      3.3.1 Cognitive thinking skills
      3.3.2 Intellectual autonomy
      3.3.3 The omnipresence of opposite point of views

      4 Conclusion
      Acknowledgement
      Appendix A Interview protocol.
      Appendix B Chinese transcript of the selected interview.
      Appendix C Example of codes and interview transcripts.
      References

    Learning and Individual Differences 20 (2010) 604–616

    Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

    Learning and Individual Differences

    journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/lindif

    Exploring cultural differences in critical thinking: Is it about my thinking style or the
    language I speak?

    Vivian Miu-Chi Lun a,b,⁎, Ronald Fischer a, Colleen Ward a

    a School of Psychology and Centre for Applied Cross-Cultural Research, Victoria University of Wellington, PO Box 600, Wellington, New Zealand
    b Department of Management, City University of Hong Kong, Tat Chee Avenue, Kowloon Tong, Kowloon, Hong Kong, China

    ⁎ Corresponding author. Department of Management
    Tat Chee Avenue, Kowloon Tong, Kowloon, Hong Kong,
    fax: +852 3442 0309.

    E-mail addresses: vivian.lun@cityu.edu.hk (V.M.-C. L
    (R. Fischer), colleen.ward@vuw.ac.nz (C. Ward).

    1041-6080/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier Inc. Al
    doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2010.07.001

    a b s t r a c t

    a r t i c l e i n f o

    Article history:
    Received 8 January 2009
    Received in revised form 29 June 2010
    Accepted 4 July 2010

    Keywords:
    Critical thinking
    Cultural differences
    English proficiency
    Dialectical thinking
    International education

    Critical thinking is deemed as an ideal in academic settings, but cultural differences in critical thinking
    performance between Asian and Western students have been reported in the international education
    literature. We examined explanations for the observed differences in critical thinking between Asian and
    New Zealand (NZ) European students, and tested hypotheses derived from research in international
    education and cultural psychology. The results showed that NZ European students performed better on two
    objective measures of critical thinking skills than Asian students. English proficiency, but not dialectical
    thinking style, could at least partially if not fully explain these differences. This finding holds with both self-
    report (Study 1) and objectively measured (Study 2a) English proficiency. The results also indicated that
    Asian students tended to rely more on dialectical thinking to solve critical thinking problems than their
    Western counterparts. In a follow-up data analysis, students’ critical thinking was found to predict
    their academic performance after controlling for the effects of English proficiency and general intellectual
    ability, but the relationship does not vary as a function of students’ cultural backgrounds or cultural adoption
    (Study 2b). Altogether, these findings contribute to our understanding of the influence of culture on critical
    thinking in international education.

    , City University of Hong Kong,
    China. Tel.: +852 3442 5679;

    un), ronald.fischer@vuw.ac.nz

    l rights reserved.

    © 2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

    1. Introduction

    Cultivation of critical thinking has been considered an important
    goal of tertiary education, especially in today’s learning environment
    where students are exposed to tremendous amount of information
    which requires effective cognitive strategies to process. Despite the
    apparent consensus of the importance of critical thinking (Pithers &
    Soden, 2000), the influence of culture on critical thinking and its
    instruction is not clear (ten Dam & Volman, 2004). In particular, there
    are debates about whether Asian students show lower level of critical
    thinking in comparison to their Western counterparts, and conse-
    quentially whether critical thinking instruction can be applied to
    students of diverse cultural backgrounds (e.g., Atkinson, 1997; Ennis,
    1998; Kumaravadivelu, 2003). The present research seeks to
    empirically address whether there is a difference in critical thinking
    between Asian and Western students, and examines the possible
    explanations and impact if such a difference exists.

    1.1. Do Asian students think less critically?

    The current trend of tertiary education involves an increasing
    number of Asian students studying at a Western English-speaking
    institution (OECD, 2009). With regard to the development of critical
    thinking in such a context, concern about whether Asian students
    tend to think less critically in contrast to their Western counterparts
    has gained much attention (Atkinson, 1997; Kumaravadivelu, 2003).

    Research showed that academic staffs experienced in teaching
    international students are dissatisfied with the international students’
    poor critical thinking and analytical skills (Robertson, Line, Jones, &
    Thomas, 2000). Academics often express that Asian students do not
    naturally take part in critical thinking because they do not overtly
    participate in classroom discussions (cf. Paton, 2005). Lee and
    Carrasquillo (2006) found that college professors perceived their Korean
    students as having “difficulty in openly expressing critical thinking”
    (p. 451). The perceptions of Asian students being less overt or less
    expressive in classrooms are often interpreted as lack of critical thinking,
    especially when Western academic standards of critical thinking
    involving overt argumentation and debate are applied (Durkin, 2008).

    The claim that Asian students think less critically has been based
    predominantly on the observations by teaching professionals of
    English-second-language (ESL)learners. Nevertheless, there iscurrently
    little empirical research directly examining differences in critical
    thinking between Asian students and their Western counterparts.

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.07.001

    mailto:vivian.lun@cityu.edu.hk

    mailto:ronald.fischer@vuw.ac.nz

    mailto:colleen.ward@vuw.ac.nz

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.07.001

    http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/10416080

    605V.M.-C. Lun et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 20 (2010) 604–616

    1.2. Critical thinking — a multi-dimensional construct

    At this point, the term critical thinking requires clarification. In the
    international education literature, the term is mostly used to refer to a
    set of behaviors which is assumed to truly reflect critical thinking
    skills (e.g., Atkinson, 1997). These behaviors often include overt
    questioning or debating of ideas in a classroom setting (Durkin, 2008).
    Teaching professionals observe these behaviors and infer whether the
    students show critical thinking. These observations then lead the
    educators to conclude whether the students have critical thinking or if
    they are critical thinkers.

    However, besides behavioral expressions, critical thinking, as a
    form of thinking, involves important cognitive skills and dispositions.
    A critical thinker is willing, persistent, flexible, open-minded and
    confident in using certain cognitive skills when it is necessary and
    appropriate (Ennis, 1987; Facione, 1990; Halpern, 1998, 2003).
    Pascarella and Terenzini (2005) summarized various definitions of
    critical thinking in the literature and suggested that college-level
    critical thinking skills include identifying assumptions behind an
    argument, recognizing important relationships, making correct
    references from data, drawing conclusions from the information or
    data provided, interpreting the merit of a conclusion based on
    available information, evaluating the credibility of a statement and its
    source, and making self-corrections. The ultimate goal of critical
    thinking is to make decisions or solve problems in different situations
    with the appropriate use of these critical thinking skills (Ennis, 1987;
    Halpern, 1998). The term critical thinking, as theorized by Ennis
    (1987) and Halpern (1998), represents a set of cognitive skills and
    dispositions which are conducive to decision making and problem
    solving in different situations.

    The concepts of critical thinking and intelligence appear similar
    and are indeed closely related, but critical thinking is more often
    accepted as teachable and improvable with appropriate instruction
    (Nickerson, Perkins, & Smith, 1985; Halpern, 2007). Nickerson et al.’s
    (1985) view of thinking skills as good strategies helps to distinguish
    between critical thinking and intelligence. Their idea was that
    thinking skills are a consequence of education, while intelligence
    relates more to the “raw power” of an individual’s mental equipment:
    “raw power is one thing and the skilled use of it (is) something else”
    (p. 44). Because it is more probable to teach skills rather than the
    supposedly unchangeable raw mental power, critical thinking has
    drawn much attention in psychology and education (e.g., Halpern,
    1998; Pithers & Soden, 2000; ten Dam & Volman, 2004).

    1.3. Focus of the present research

    The debate in the international education literature has mainly
    been focused on the behavioral expression of critical thinking (e.g.,
    Atkinson, 1997; Kumaravadivelu, 2003). Nevertheless, observation of
    classroom behaviors is susceptible to the influence of different
    cultural rules and assumptions underlying behaviors such as talking
    (Kim, 2002) or debating and questioning authorities (Tweed &
    Lehman, 2002). Here we focus on critical thinking as a skill because
    (a) the cultivation of critical thinking skills is a principal goal of
    university education (Pithers & Soden, 2000), and (b) these skills can
    be objectively measured.

    1.4. Cultural differences in critical thinking

    To examine if Asian students really think less critically compared
    with their Western counterparts, one of the best ways is to test and
    compare the critical thinking skills between these two groups of
    students. Nevertheless, as ten Dam and Volman (2004) observed,
    empirical research which addresses the role of culture in relation to
    critical thinking is currently limited. To the best of our knowledge, there
    is no published study involving direct comparison of critical thinking

    skills betweenAsian and Westernstudents in aninternational education
    setting. To fill this gap in the literature, the present study directly tests
    whether there are objective differences in critical thinking skills
    between Asian and Western students who are studying at a Western
    institution.

    Based on the assumption that the observations made by university
    teaching professionals (e.g., Lee & Carrasquillo, 2006; Robertson et al.,
    2000) reflect a kernel of truth, we hypothesize that Western students
    would perform better than Asian students in an objective measure of
    critical thinking (Hypothesis 1).

    1.5. Dialectical thinking

    The literature on dialectical thinking proposed one causal
    mechanism to explain why critical thinking performances may be
    lower among Asian students compared to Western students (Nisbett,
    Peng, Choi, & Norenzayan, 2001; Peng & Nisbett, 1999). According to
    this paradigm, the Asian way of information processing can be
    summarized by three principles: 1) reality is dynamic and change-
    able; 2) opposing propositions may exist in the same object or event;
    and 3) everything in life and nature is related (Nisbett et al., 2001).
    Asians, compared with Westerners, tend to perceive more changes,
    are more tolerant to contradictions, and perceive things as more
    interrelated. These three characteristics involved in the Asian way of
    thinking are suggested to be incongruent with the formal logical
    tradition of thinking which is dominant in Western cultures (Peng &
    Nisbett, 1999).

    These cultural thinking styles have been shown to have important
    influence on certain psychological and behavioral differences be-
    tween Asian and Westerners. In a seminal study, Norenzayan, Smith,
    Kim, and Nisbett (2002) found that Westerners were more likely to
    use formal logical rules in reasoning, whereas East Asians used more
    intuitive and experience-based reasoning when there was a conflict
    between intuitive and formal reasoning strategies. In one of their
    experiments, European American and Korean students were pre-
    sented with a set of arguments and asked to evaluate whether or not a
    conclusion followed logically from the premises for each argument.
    When faced with a conclusion that logically follows the arguments but
    is intuitively unbelievable, Korean students tended to make more
    mistakes in judging the conclusion as invalid than the American
    students, indicating that they relied more on their own experience but
    less on formal logical rules in deductive reasoning than their
    American counterparts. Norenzayan et al. (2002) speculated that
    the pedagogical emphasis on critical thinking in Western classrooms
    compared with the experience-based approach in Asian classrooms
    may be one of the reasons of the different modes of thinking in the
    two cultural systems. Therefore, these laboratory experiments
    suggested that dialectical thinking and critical thinking are linked,
    but in an opposite direction. A preference for dialectical thinking may
    prompt Asians students to seek a “middle-way” between apparent
    contradictions more than their Western counterparts (Peng & Nisbett,
    1999, 2000) and to choose intuitive reasoning over formal reasoning
    (Norenzayan et al., 2002), which then may explain the noted
    difference on critical thinking performance between Asian and
    Western students. Therefore, we expect that dialectical thinking
    style mediates the differences in critical thinking skills between Asian
    and Western students (Hypothesis 2).

    This hypothesis is based on the assumption that dialectical thinking is
    applicable to both Western and Asian students. Based on the different
    historical developments of philosophical thoughts in the East and the
    West, dialecticalthinking is suggested to be more typicalofAsiancultures
    compared to Western contexts, where analytical thinking styles are more
    typical (Nisbett et al., 2001). Individuals within each of these cultural
    contexts differ in terms of their propensity to engage in each of these
    thinking styles. In other words, dialectical thinking is treated as an
    individual difference variable that shows variability across cultural

    606 V.M.-C. Lun et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 20 (2010) 604–616

    contexts and can be used to explain differences in objective measures of
    critical thinking (Matsumoto & Yoo, 2006).

    Using individual difference variables to account for observed cultural
    differences has become increasingly popular because of its effectiveness
    to account for the active ingredients of cultural differences in
    psychological variables (Leung & van de Vijver, 2008). However, this
    approach assumes that the mediating variable is similarly related to the
    target variable in all cultural groups involved. Although this rationale
    underlies the development of individual difference measures of
    dialectical thinking, and these scales have been used in past research
    to explain cultural differences in other psychological variables and
    behaviors (e.g., Hamamura, Heine, & Paulhus, 2008; Spencer-Rodgers,
    Peng, Wang, & Hou, 2004), it is uncertain whether dialectical thinking
    style is similarly associated with critical thinking skills in both European
    and Asian student populations. Peng and Nisbett (1999, 2000)
    suggested that dialectical thinking style reflects philosophical tradition
    prevalent in Eastern cultures, whereas Western culture is characterized
    by analytical thinking style, which has a root of Greek philosophical
    tradition. Therefore, these thinking styles may be qualitatively different
    and may not relate to critical thinking in one cultural group or another.
    Such qualitative differences in their functioning can weaken the
    usefulness of thinking styles in accounting for cultural differences in
    critical thinking. Therefore, a secondary aim of our analysis is to explore
    whether dialectical thinking shows the same relationship with critical
    thinking in both Asian and European student populations. The findings
    will have important implications for the conceptualization of dialectical
    thinking across cultural groups.

    1.6. English proficiency

    An alternative hypothesis for explaining cultural difference in
    critical thinking relates to Asian students’ is the use of English as a
    second language (Paton, 2005). In an international education setting,
    Asian international or immigrant students usually possess lower level
    of English proficiency than their native English-speaking counter-
    parts. As a result, these Asian students may be more “cognitively
    overloaded” when using English in tasks requiring critical thinking.
    English proficiency then accounts for their lower critical thinking
    performance. Campbell, Adams, and Davis (2007) suggested similar
    processes of increased cognitive demand for ESL students in solving
    mathematical problems. According to the cognitive load theory (Paas,
    Renkl, & Sweller, 2003), only limited amount of information can be
    stored and processed in working memory. Using this theoretical
    framework, Campbell et al. (2007) illustrated that ESL learners
    required some of that limited working memory to process informa-
    tion related to the unfamiliar language and structure of word
    problems, which overloaded their working memory and prevented
    them from effectively processing information pertinent to the
    question per se. Similar ideas can be applied to ESL learners who
    are required to think critically about problems presented in English.

    In fact, the use of a second language has been shown to have
    detrimental effect on one’s performance in cognitive tasks. Takano and
    Noda (1993) observed that native-Japanese speakers who had to use
    English as their second language to work on a linguistic task performed
    less well in a concurrent calculation task than they would in using
    Japanese on the linguistic task. The same deteriorating effect was also
    found among native English speakers who had to use Japanese on the
    linguistic task. In light of these findings, Takano and Noda (1993)
    suggested that the use of a foreign language would cause temporary
    decline of thinking ability as a result of heavier cognitive processing load.

    Language proficiency has been found to play an important role in
    students’ critical thinking performance. Previous research on the
    predictors of university students’ critical thinking test performance
    showed that verbal scores in the Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT) were
    significantly and positively related to critical thinking skills as well as
    grade point average (Taube, 1997). Clifford, Boufal, and Kurtz (2004)

    also observed college students’ scores on the verbal comprehension
    subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition (WAIS-III;
    Wechsler, 1997) being significant predictor of their critical thinking test
    scores. Although these studies did not address the issue of culture, the
    findings suggested a positive relationship between language proficiency
    and critical thinking performance, which may be used to explain the
    difference in critical thinking between Asian and Western students.
    Accordingly, we hypothesize that English proficiency explains cultural
    differences in critical thinking (Hypothesis 3).

    1.7. Possible consequence of cultural differences in critical thinking

    An important implication of observing cultural difference in
    critical thinking concerns the practice of critical thinking in interna-
    tional education. If Asian students were indeed less capable of critical
    thinking than their Western counterparts, instructions emphasizing
    critical thinking might become something undesirable to Asian
    students. The increasing number of Asian students studying in
    Western institutions (OECD, 2009) might then undermine the values
    of cultivating critical thinking in university education (see Atkinson,
    1997; Davidson, 1998; Ennis, 1998; Gieve, 1998; and Kumaravadivelu,
    2003 for details on this line of debate).

    If a course has been designed to promote critical thinking,
    students’ critical thinking skills should be reflected in their perfor-
    mance in the course. That is, students should be able to practice their
    critical thinking in course assessments. Previous research in the
    United States showed that students’ critical thinking skills measured
    by standardized instruments were positively related to their academic
    achievement in courses which emphasize the practice of critical
    thinking (e.g., Bowles, 2000; Collins & Onwuegbuzie, 2000). If such
    positive relationship between critical thinking skills and academic
    performance is equally applicable to both Asian and Western student
    samples, the emphasis of critical thinking can then be considered
    equally useful to both groups of students.

    Assuming Asian students are indeed less capable of critical thinking
    than Western students, in a course which emphasizes the use of critical
    thinking, it would be possible to observe that Asian students showing
    lower level of academic performance (Hypothesis 4). Nevertheless, ample
    research evidence showed that Asian students outperform their Western
    counterparts in academic achievements in different national and
    international research, even though certain Asian learning practices
    have been considered unfavorable to good learning outcomes according
    to Western standards (i.e., “the paradox of the Asian learners”, see
    Watkins & Biggs, 2001 for details). Therefore, it would also be possible
    that Asian students can achieve high grades in courses emphasizing
    critical thinking despite their apparently lower level of critical thinking
    skills. Instead of observing systematic cultural difference in academic
    achievement, the relationship between critical thinking and academic
    performance might be found to be weaker among Asian students than
    Western students (Hypothesis 5). In such case, the relevance of
    emphasizing critical thinking and the usefulness of related educational
    practices in international tertiary education would still require
    reexamination.

    1.8. Overview of the present research

    In the present research, we are testing whether there are differences
    between Asian and Western students on two different objective tests of
    critical thinking. Second, we explore the roles of English language ability
    and dialectical thinking styles, based on the propositions of the
    Cognitive Load Theory (Paas et al., 2003) and the dialectical thinking
    paradigm (Nisbett et al., 2001; Peng & Nisbett, 1999), in explaining the
    observed difference. The relationship between dialectical thinking and
    critical thinking is also explored. Finally, to understand how a teaching
    programme emphasizing critical thinking may be influenced by culture,
    we examine the relationship between critical thinking and academic

    607V.M.-C. Lun et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 20 (2010) 604–616

    performance in both Asian and Western student samples. The finding
    from this investigation is expected to provide important empirical data
    about the potential impact of culture on the emphasis of critical thinking
    in university education.

    The research was conducted in a large New Zealand University. New
    Zealand provides a good case study due to its cultural diversity. In the
    past 20 years, the growth in Asian population has been the highest
    among all immigrating ethnicities in New Zealand (Friesen, 2008). In
    the educational context, New Zealand attracts students mostly from
    Asian countries and communities such as China, Korea, and Japan (New
    Zealand Ministry of Education, 2008). This characteristic of the students’
    cultural composition in New Zealand offers an interesting avenue to test
    out the aforementioned research hypotheses.

    Two studies were conducted in this research. Study 1 was designed
    to provide an initial account of whether there is a difference in critical
    thinking abilities between the Chinese and New Zealand European
    students. A newly-developed critical thinking instrument, the Halpern
    Critical Thinking Assessment using Everyday Situations (HCTAES;
    Halpern, 2006), was used to assess the critical thinking skills of a
    sample of Chinese and European students. The roles of dialectical
    thinking and English proficiency in the observed cross-cultural
    difference were also explored.

    Study 2 included an assessment with a more popular measure of
    critical thinking (the Watson–Glaser Critical Thinking Appraisal Short
    Form; Watson & Glaser, 1994, 2006). The study replicated the design
    and tested the same set of hypotheses as in Study 1, except that a
    different instrument was used to enhance the generalizability of the
    results. Following the research strategies employed in previous studies
    on the predictors of critical thinking performance (e.g., Clifford et al.,
    2004; Taube, 1997), an objective measure which assesses English
    proficiency and general intellectual ability was also included. A follow-
    up data analysis of a subset of study 2 focusing on the academic
    performance of students in a management class was conducted to test
    hypotheses 4 and 5, addressing concerns about the possible conse-
    quence of cultural difference in critical thinking in international
    education.

    2. Study 1: pilot study with the HCTAES

    This pilot study was conducted to provide preliminary evidence on
    the observed cross-cultural differences in critical thinking between
    Asian and Western students. To explore the effect of dialectical thinking
    and English proficiency in explaining the differences, self-report
    measures of these variables were included in the assessment.

    2.1. Method

    2.1.1. Participants
    Seventy students of a university in New Zealand participated in the

    study. Twenty-four students identified themselves as ethnically
    Chinese, 35 as New Zealand European, and 11 participants were of
    other ethnic identities such as New Zealand Maori and Samoan. Because
    our research focus was on Asian and New Zealand European students,
    only the data from the Chinese and New Zealand European students
    were subjected to further analysis. All international students had to
    provide evidence of sufficient proficiency in written and spoken English
    upon enrolling to the university. Among the 59 students included, there
    were 47 females, 11 males, and one unidentified. The average age of
    these students was 21.95 years (SD=1.99).

    2.1.2. Materials
    Participants were instructed to complete a set of instruments as

    described below:

    2.1.2.1. Halpern Critical Thinking Assessment using Everyday Situations
    (HCTAES). The HCTAES (Halpern, 2006) is an instrument designed for

    assessing critical thinking abilities on five dimensions, namely, verbal
    reasoning skills, argument analysis skills, skills in thinking as
    hypothesis testing, using likelihood and uncertainty, and decision
    making and problem-solving skills, which are the five important
    college-level critical thinking skills according to Halpern (1998). The
    test consists of 25 everyday-life scenarios each with one open-ended
    item and one forced-choice item.

    According to the information in the test manual, the HCTAES was
    shown to correlate positively with the Arlin Test of Formal Reasoning
    (Arlin, 1984) within a high-school student sample and a college
    student sample (r=.32 in both samples). The Arlin Test is a test of
    formal reasoning based on Piaget’s model of cognitive development.
    In addition, the HCTAES was found to have a positive correlation
    (about .60) with the Analytic subtest of the Graduate Record Exam,
    showing convergent validity with other tests which measured related
    cognitive abilities.

    The whole test takes about 90 min to finish, so to keep the testing
    time reasonably brief for the participants, only the forced-choice
    portion of the test was used. The use of both open-ended and close-
    ended portions together was intended for educational and training
    purpose. However, the forced-choice portion alone would be
    sufficient for a brief assessment of the critical thinking abilities of
    the samples, which was intended to show if test-takers are able to use
    the skill when they are provided with more clues in the close-ended
    items (Halpern, 2006).

    Therefore in the final test, there were altogether 25 forced-choice
    scenario based items, with five items tapping each skill category. In
    addition to the more common multiple-choice format which involves
    choosing the best answer among a number of alternatives (nine
    items), there were also items requiring the testees to give ratings on
    different options pertinent to a particular scenario (seven items), to
    choose multiple correct answers among a number of alternatives (five
    items), and to identify the characteristics of a list of statements (four
    items). Given the diversified response formats of the items, the final
    score was calculated using the standardized scores of every item; a
    similar procedure was employed by Hau, Ho, Lai, Ku, and Hui (2008).

    2.1.2.2. Dialectical Self Scale (DSS). The DSS was developed by Spencer-
    Rodgers et al. (2001) for measuring dialectical thinking in the domain
    of self-perception. It has been utilized to explain differences in
    psychological well-being (Spencer-Rodgers et al., 2004) and response
    styles (Hamamura et al., 2008) between Asian and Western cultures.
    The scale was constructed using a 7-point format with higher score
    indicating higher level of dialectical self-concept. Sample items
    included “When I hear two sides of an argument, I often agree with
    both”, “I often find that my beliefs and attitudes will change under
    different contexts”. Cronbach’s alpha was .78 in the Chinese sample
    and .71 in the New Zealand European sample.

    2.1.2.3. Perceived proficiency in reading and writing English (English).
    The participants were asked to rate their proficiency in English on a 7-
    point scale on “How proficient are you in reading in English” and
    “How proficient are you in writing in English”, with higher scores
    indicating higher level of proficiency. The correlation between these
    items was .82 (pb.01) in the Chinese sample and .92 (pb.01) in the
    New Zealand European sample. The scores on these items were
    averaged to provide an estimate of the participants’ perceived
    proficiency in reading and writing English.

    All measures were administered in English because English is the
    medium of instruction in the New Zealand tertiary education system.
    Entry requirements of the universities in New Zealand include
    providing evidence of the students’ English language proficiency,
    either in the form of the NZ Bursary Examination or standardized
    international English proficiency tests. Therefore we can confidently
    assume that the participants possess a reasonable level of English
    proficiency to take part, and no participant from either sample

    Table 1
    Descriptive statistics and independent sample t-tests of the target variables in Study 1.

    Chinese New Zealand
    European

    t-test

    M SD M SD

    HCTAES (standardized) −1.26 1.70 0.87 1.13 −5.78⁎⁎⁎
    Perceived English proficiency 4.58 1.10 6.71 0.61 −8.63⁎⁎⁎
    DSS 4.00 0.52 3.87 0.46 1.25

    ⁎⁎⁎ pb.001.

    608 V.M.-C. Lun et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 20 (2010) 604–616

    reported difficulty in understanding the instructions or the items. The
    average time required for completing the session was about one hour.

    2.1.3. Analytical strategy
    To examine the effect of perceived English proficiency and the DSS

    in the relationship between culture and critical thinking skills,
    mediation analysis was used (MacKinnon, Fairchild, & Fritz, 2007).
    In testing the mediation hypotheses, the procedures proposed by
    Preacher and Hayes (2004, 2008) on testing indirect effects were used
    instead of the more commonly adopted Baron and Kenny’s (1986)
    causal-step procedures with regressions or Structural Equation
    Modeling (SEM).

    According to Preacher and Hayes (2004), the method described by
    Baron and Kenny (1986) is likely to suffer from low statistical power,
    especially in small samples (MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West, &
    Sheets, 2002). For example, the Baron and Kenny procedure requires
    significance of the X–Y relation. However, the coefficient may be
    nonsignificant due to low statistical power even though a nonzero
    effect in the population is actually present. In this situation, mediation
    model cannot be tested based on the Baron and Kenny’s procedures
    which then results in a Type II error. Instead, Preacher and Hayes
    (2004) suggested that testing the significance of the indirect effect
    between X and Y through the mediator requires one fewer hypothesis
    test, which reduces the likelihood of Type II error in mediation
    analysis. The Sobel test (Sobel, 1982) has been commonly used for
    testing significance of indirect effect (MacKinnon et al., 2002).
    However, the test requires the estimates of indirect effect to be
    normally distributed which is rarely the case in small samples. To
    circumvent the problem associated with small sample sizes, Preacher
    and Hayes (2004) recommended a bootstrapping procedure which
    makes no assumption about the sampling distribution of the
    estimates of effects.

    Bootstrapping involves repeated sampling from the data set with
    replacement and estimates the indirect effect in each resampled data
    set (Preacher & Hayes, 2004). An approximation of the sampling
    distribution of the indirect effect will be built by repeating the
    resampling process for thousands of times and then be used to
    construct confidence intervals for the indirect effect. The procedures
    of testing indirect effects with bootstrapping have been extended to
    multiple mediator models, with syntaxes and macros being designed
    for related analysis with common statistical software such as SPSS,
    SAS, and LISREL (Preacher & Hayes, 2008). The bootstrapping
    procedure generates estimates of the total effect of X on Y, the direct
    effect of X on Y after entering the list of mediators, the indirect effects
    of X on Y via the list of mediators, and the specific indirect effects of X
    on Y via each particular mediator. The total indirect effect of X on Y is
    the sum of all specific indirect effects and is equal to the difference
    between total and direct effects.

    The SPSS macro offered by Preacher and Hayes (2008) was used in
    the present analysis. Each of the total and specific indirect effects
    generated through the procedure were assessed by three 95%
    bootstrap confidence intervals (CIs), namely, the percentile, the
    bias-corrected (BC), and the bias-corrected and accelerated (BCa)
    intervals. An indirect effect is shown to be significantly different from
    zero at 95% confidence if zero is not included in the range of CIs.

    2.2. Results and discussion

    2.2.1. Cultural differences in critical thinking skills and other self-report
    measures

    Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics and independent sample t-
    tests results of the three variables under consideration. Significant
    difference in critical thinking skills was noted between the two
    samples, with New Zealand European students performing better
    than the Chinese students in the HCTAES. This result supports
    Hypothesis 1 which was about the difference in critical thinking
    between Asian and Westerns students.

    The two samples were also significantly different from each other
    in terms of their perceived English proficiency, again with New
    Zealand European students scoring significantly higher than the
    Chinese sample. However, the two samples did not significantly differ
    from each other on the DSS.

    2.2.2. Mediation between culture and critical thinking skills
    Multiple mediation analysis was conducted following the proce-

    dures suggested by Preacher and Hayes (2008). Perceived English
    proficiency and the DSS were tested as mediators of the relationship
    between culture and critical thinking skills. The effect of gender was
    controlled for in the analysis.

    The total effect of culture on critical thinking skills was significant
    b=2.45, SE=0.34, pb .001. The partial effect of gender on critical
    thinking skills was not significant (b=0.38, SE=0.41, p=.35),
    indicating that there is no significant gender effect on the measure
    of critical thinking skills. With consideration of the two mediators in
    the model, the direct effect of culture on critical thinking skills became
    only marginally significant, b=1.00, SE=0.53, p=.06. Using the logic
    of Baron and Kenny’s (1986) causal-step procedures, the reduction of
    the coefficient between culture and critical thinking skills indicated
    that at least some of the proposed mediating variables in the model
    were potent.

    Table 2 summarizes the result of the bootstrapping mediation
    analysis of the total and specific indirect effects as well as contrasts
    between the specific indirect effects. As zero is not contained in the
    confidence intervals of the total indirect effect, the total indirect effect
    was significantly different from zero, indicating that the relationship
    between culture and critical thinking skills was mediated by at least
    some of the proposed mediators. The specific indirect effect of
    perceived English proficiency was the only one significantly different
    from zero at 95% confidence, showing that perceived English
    proficiency was the significant mediator in the model. These results
    provided support to Hypothesis 3 but not Hypothesis 2.

    The results of this pilot study showed that there was a difference
    between Chinese and New Zealand European student samples on
    critical thinking performance as assessed by an objective measure,
    with the latter sample performing better than the former in the test.
    Self-report English proficiency could, at least to a certain extent,
    explain this difference. Nevertheless, dialectical thinking style could
    not explain the cultural difference in critical thinking as we proposed.

    3. Study 2a: assessment with the WGCTA-SF

    To test the research hypotheses more fully, an assessment was
    conducted with another instrument — the Watson–Glaser Critical
    Thinking Appraisal Short Form (WGCTA-SF; Watson & Glaser, 1994,
    2006). The WGCTA is one of the most popular measures of critical
    thinking. Similar to the HCTAES (Halpern, 2006), the WGCTA employs
    everyday situations to test critical thinking in a general sense (Renaud
    & Murray, 2008). Following the research strategy employed in Taube
    (1997) and Clifford et al. (2004), English proficiency and general
    intellectual ability are assessed with an objective measure: The
    Shipley Institute of Living Scale (SILS; Shipley, 1940; Zachary, 1991).
    We expected that an objective measure of English proficiency would

    Table 2
    Mediation analysis in Study 1.

    Bootstrapping results for indirect effects

    Point
    Estimate

    Percentile
    95% CI

    BC
    95% CI

    BCa
    95% CI

    Lower Upper Lower Upper Lower Upper

    Indirect effects
    English 1.41 0.74 2.22 0.74 2.22 0.72 2.19
    DSS 0.02 −0.08 0.21 −0.05 0.28 −0.05 0.26
    Total 1.44 0.75 2.28 0.73 2.24 0.70 2.22

    Contrasts
    English vs. DSS 1.39 0.69 2.21 0.70 2.23 0.68 2.19

    5000 bootstrap samples; significant indirect effects or contrasts in bold.

    609V.M.-C. Lun et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 20 (2010) 604–616

    provide more solid evidence of the role of language in explaining
    cultural difference in critical thinking. We controlled for general
    intellectual ability, to rule out the possibility that differences in critical
    thinking are due to the difference in basal general intellectual
    competence between the two samples.

    Apart from these additional control measures, we acknowledge
    that self-report English proficiency, apart from representing the
    intended perceived English proficiency, reflects also a person’s
    adoption to an English-speaking cultural environment (e.g., Gaudet
    & Clément, 2009). Because of this possible linkage between perceived
    language proficiency and acculturation, it is logical to argue that the
    findings in Study 1 was essentially showing that adoption of an
    English-speaking culture, rather than proficiency in using English, was
    what explained the cultural difference in critical thinking skills.1 In
    such case, critical thinking might alternatively be interpreted as
    something specifically favorable to Western culture, thereby under-
    mining the usefulness of practicing critical thinking in an interna-
    tional education context. To rule out the possibility of adoption to a
    Western culture was the actual factor leading to cultural differences in
    critical thinking, a measure of cultural adoption was also included in
    the analysis.

    Around half of the participants in this study were recruited
    through a Management course as a part of the accreditation project of
    the Management school. Data on the final course grade was available
    for this subsample. As suggested above, cultural differences observed
    in critical thinking have important implication in evaluating the
    suitability of promoting critical thinking in an international class-
    room. If teaching professionals attempt to promote critical thinking in
    their courses but students of different cultural backgrounds cannot
    equally apply their critical thinking as intended, the usefulness of
    promoting critical thinking in education would then be questionable.
    Therefore, this data set with students’ course grades enables the
    possibility of testing the relationship between students’ academic
    performance and critical thinking abilities, and thereby provides
    information about the practical implication of promoting critical
    thinking in higher education.

    3.1. Method

    3.1.1. Participants
    Three hundred and sixty-six university students participated in

    the study, among these participants 102 identified themselves as
    ethnically Asian, 210 identified as ethnically New Zealand European,
    and 54 of other ethnicities, including New Zealand Maori, Samoan,
    African, and mixed ethnicities. Again, because the major focus of this
    research was on the comparison between Asian and New Zealand
    European students, the data from those of the other ethnicities were

    1 We would like to thank an anonymous reviewer of an earlier version of this
    manuscript for pointing out this possible alternative explanation.

    not included in the subsequent analysis. The majority of the Asian
    sample consisted of Chinese (68.6%), followed by Indian (9.8%),
    Vietnamese (6.9%), and Filipinos (3.9%), with the rest being
    Cambodian, Indonesian, Japanese, Korean, Malay, Sri Lankan, and
    Thai. This pattern closely resembles the actual situation of interna-
    tional education in New Zealand, where Chinese migrants form the
    majority of Asian students (New Zealand Ministry of Education,
    2008). The average age of the final sample of 312 participants was
    20.09 years (SD=4.83), with 124 males, 187 females and one
    unidentified gender.

    It is noted that the Asian student sample appeared to be more
    diverse than the NZ European sample. Nevertheless, because we are
    looking for the active ingredient of cultural difference in critical
    thinking by using individual differences variables (Matsumoto & Yoo,
    2006), the differences in terms of within-group diversity between the
    two samples would be accounted for in the analysis. Therefore, the
    conclusion drawn would not be affected by the seemingly more
    diversified Asian sample.

    3.1.2. Materials
    The English version of the following instruments was administered

    to the participants. The average time required for completing the
    whole session was about one hour.

    3.1.2.1. Watson–Glaser Critical Thinking Appraisal Short Form (WGCTA-
    SF). The WGCTA was designed to measure critical thinking abilities
    along five dimensions, namely, inferences, recognition of assump-
    tions, deduction, interpretation, and evaluations of argument. The
    short form version with 40 items was employed in this study (Watson
    & Glaser, 1994, 2006). The WGCTA-SF was derived by shortening the
    original 80-item Form A of the WGCTA (Watson & Glaser, 1980) in an
    attempt to reduce the administration time. Reliability of the test was
    shown by Cronbach’s alpha of .81 based on a sample of 1608
    participants in the original development of the short form, and alphas
    between .66 and .85 in the other studies reported in the Manual.
    Validity of the test was demonstrated by the significant correlation
    between test scores and criterion-related measures such as effective-
    ness in clinical decision making (Shin, 1998) and cognitive problem-
    solving skills (Spector, Schneider, Vance, & Hazlett, 2000). In each
    subscale, directions and sample questions were provided in front of
    the actual test items. All participants were instructed to read carefully
    before answering the questions.

    Although the scores on the WGCTA-SF might be interpreted at the
    subscale level, a meta-analytic review on the psychometric properties
    of the WGCTA showed that it would be better to treat the scale as a
    measure of general critical thinking competency but not to interpret
    the subscales individually (Bernard et al., 2008). This suggestion was
    based on the principal component analysis of the subscale means of
    the original versions of WGCTA (Watson & Glaser, 1980) which
    yielded a one-factor solution. In view of the empirical evidence and for
    the sake of parsimony, only the total score of WGCTA-SF is interpreted
    in the present research.

    3.1.2.2. Shipley Institute of Living Scale (SILS). Developed by Shipley in
    1940, the SILS was intended for assessing general intelligence in
    adults and adolescents. The revised version by Zachary (1991) was
    employed in this study. The SILS consists of two subtests, a 40-item
    vocabulary test and a 20-item abstraction test. In the vocabulary test,
    participants were asked to choose among four alternative words that
    mean the same or nearly the same to a specific target word. In the
    abstraction test, a logical sequence was presented and the participants
    were required to complete the sequence by filling in the appropriate
    numbers or letters. Although the test scores could be used together to
    estimate IQ scores based on more popular intelligence testing
    instruments such as the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised
    (WAIS-R; Wechsler, 1981) by means of various sophisticated

    Table 3
    Descriptive statistics and independent sample t-tests of the target variables in Study 2a.

    Asian New Zealand
    European

    t-test
    M SD M SD

    WGCTA total 24.07 5.64 27.05 6.19 −4.02⁎⁎⁎
    SILS vocabulary test 24.15 5.90 29.08 4.13 −7.55⁎⁎⁎
    SILS abstraction test 16.15 3.00 16.53 2.24 −1.14
    DSS 4.02 0.46 3.92 0.56 1.74
    Cultural adoption 4.20 1.02 5.26 0.67 −9.29⁎⁎⁎
    Perceived English Proficiency 5.17 1.45 6.62 0.68 −9.56⁎⁎⁎

    ⁎⁎⁎ pb.001.

    Table 4
    Mediation analysis in Study 2a.

    Point
    Estimate
    Bootstrapping results for indirect effects
    Percentile
    95% CI
    BC
    95% CI
    BCa
    95% CI
    Lower Upper Lower Upper Lower Upper

    Indirect effects
    SILS vocabulary 2.21 1.39 3.15 1.42 3.18 1.42 3.18
    DSS 0.02 −0.11 0.18 −0.07 0.24 −0.07 0.24
    Cultural adoption −0.57 −1.54 0.19 −1.51 0.21 −1.42 0.27
    Perceived English −0.47 −1.50 0.56 −1.51 0.56 −1.53 0.56
    Total 1.19 0.16 2.24 0.21 2.33 0.22 2.34

    Contrasts
    SILS vocabulary vs.

    DSS
    2.19 1.38 3.11 1.42 3.17 1.42 3.17

    SILS vocabulary vs.
    cultural adoption

    2.78 1.67 4.10 1.65 4.09 1.61 4.06

    SILS vocabulary vs.
    Perceived English

    2.67 1.15 4.32 1.20 4.39 1.20 4.39

    DSS vs. cultural
    adoption

    0.59 −0.16 1.56 −0.18 1.53 −0.26 1.43

    DSS vs. Perceived
    English

    0.49 −0.57 1.56 −0.56 1.57 −0.55 1.58

    Cultural adoption vs.
    Perceived English

    −0.11 −1.87 1.39 −1.85 1.41 −1.76 1.49

    5000 bootstrap samples; significant indirect effects or contrasts in bold.

    610 V.M.-C. Lun et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 20 (2010) 604–616

    conversion procedures (e.g., Paulson & Lin, 1970; Zachary, Paulson, &
    Gorsuch, 1985), the raw scores of each test were used as measures of
    the participants’ English proficiency (vocabulary score) and general
    intellectual competence (abstraction score), respectively.

    3.1.2.3. Dialectical Self Scale (DSS). The same instrument employed in
    Study 1 was used. Cronbach’s alpha was .65 in the Asian sample and
    .78 in the New Zealand European sample.

    3.1.2.4. Perceived English proficiency (English). The two items used in
    Study 1 were again used to indicate perceived English proficiency.
    Correlation between the two items was .93 (pb.01) in the Asian
    sample and.76 (pb.01) in the New Zealand European sample. The
    scores on those items were averaged to estimate the participants’
    perceived proficiency in reading and writing English.

    3.1.2.5. Cultural adoption of behavioral norms in New Zealand. The
    Behavioral Acculturation Scale (BACS-16) was a 16-item scale
    developed by Groenvynck, Beirens, Arends-Toth, and Fontaine
    (2006) to measure two independent dimensions of acculturation,
    namely, cultural maintenance and cultural adoption. The term
    acculturation is used to describe the cultural change that a person
    undergoes during cross-cultural transitions (Ward, 1996). Based on
    the two-dimensional model of acculturation (Berry, 1997), the BACS-
    16 was designed to capture an individual’s cultural change in terms of
    their actual knowledge, actions, and behaviors related to their home
    and host cultures. In essence, the scale consists of eight pairs of item.
    Within each pair, one item focuses on the maintenance of home
    culture, while the other item focuses on the adoption of host culture.

    In the present study, eight items from the cultural adoption
    subscale were chosen to capture the participants’ adoption of
    behavioral norms in New Zealand. All participants were instructed
    to rate the items concerning their knowledge of and behaviors within
    New Zealand culture using a 6-point Likert scale, ranging from fully
    disagree (1) to fully agree (6). Sample items include “I know the New
    Zealand culture and traditions well” and “I live according to rules that
    apply in the New Zealand culture”. Cronbach’s alpha was .87 in the
    Asian sample and .86 in the New Zealand European sample.

    3.2. Results and discussion

    3.2.1. Cultural differences in WGCTA, SILS scores, and other self-report
    measures

    Table 3 shows the descriptive statistics of the target variables in
    the Asian and New Zealand European samples. It was observed that
    the New Zealand European sample scored higher on all variables
    except the DSS. Independent sample t-tests on the variables revealed
    that the differences between the two samples were statistically
    significant in terms of the WGCTA score, the SILS vocabulary score,
    cultural adoption, and perceived English proficiency. The differences
    observed in SILS abstraction score and DSS were not statistically
    significant, indicating that the two cultural groups are not signifi-

    cantly different from each other in general intellectual competence
    and self-report dialectical thinking style.

    3.2.2. Mediation between culture and critical thinking skills
    Similar to the pilot study, multiple mediation analysis was

    conducted using the procedures proposed by Preacher and Hayes
    (2008). SILS abstraction score was included as a covariate to control
    for the effect of general intellectual competence on critical thinking
    skills. Gender was also controlled for in the analysis.

    The total effect of culture on the WGCTA score was significant
    b=2.70, SE=0.72, pb.001. The partial effect of gender on the WGCTA
    was not significant (b=−0.19, SE=0.67, p=.78), indicating that
    there was no significant gender effect on critical thinking skills as
    measured by the WGCTA. However, the partial effect of SILS
    abstraction score was significant (b=0.53, SE=0.13, pb .001),
    suggesting that general intellectual competence was positively
    related to critical thinking skills. After taking into account the
    mediators, the direct effect of culture on critical thinking skills
    became statistically nonsignificant, b=1.51, SE=0.86, p=.08. Again,
    based on Baron and Kenny’s (1986) logic, the reduction in the effect of
    culture on the WGCTA indicated that at least some of the proposed
    mediators were potent.

    Table 4 summarizes the results of the bootstrapped mediation
    analysis of the total and specific indirect effects as well as contrasts
    between the specific indirect effects. Consistent with the observation
    on the difference between the total effect and the direct effect of
    culture on critical thinking, zero was not contained in the confidence
    intervals of the total indirect effect, which showed that the total
    indirect effect was significantly different from zero. In other words,
    the relationship between culture and critical thinking skills was
    mediated by at least some of the proposed mediators.

    Inspection of the CIs of the proposed mediators revealed that only
    the specific indirect effect of SILS vocabulary was significantly
    different from zero. Tests of contrasts between the specific indirect
    effects showed that the specific indirect effect of SILS vocabulary was
    significantly different from those of the other variables, suggesting
    that it was the most important mediator in the relationship between
    culture and critical thinking skills among the variables considered.

    Table 5
    Correlation among the variables in the two samples in Study 2a.

    WGCTA SILS
    vocabulary

    SILS
    abstraction

    DSS

    Cultural
    adoption

    SILS vocabulary (.43⁎⁎)
    .44⁎⁎

    SILS abstraction (.43⁎⁎) (.33⁎⁎)
    .32⁎⁎ .36⁎⁎

    DSS (.26⁎) (.20) (.29⁎⁎)
    −.04 .05 .03

    Cultural adoption (.04) (.32⁎⁎) (.05) (−.09)
    −.12 −.05 −.07 .02

    Perceived English
    proficiency

    (.13) (.51⁎⁎) (.12) (−.04) (.64⁎⁎)
    .06 .20⁎⁎ .19⁎ −.04 .12

    Numbers in parentheses are correlations in the Asian sample, n=94; Numbers without
    parentheses are correlations in the New Zealand European sample, n=169; listwise
    deletion of missing data.
    ⁎ pb.05.
    ⁎⁎ pb .01.

    611V.M.-C. Lun et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 20 (2010) 604–616

    Therefore, hypothesis 3 was supported with an objective measure of
    English proficiency.

    It is noted that the specific indirect effect of perceived English
    proficiency, unlike that in Study 1, was not significant in the current
    analysis. A subsequent analysis using only DSS and perceived English
    proficiency as mediators revealed that the specific indirect effect of
    self-report English proficiency was significant without the SILS
    vocabulary score in the list of mediators.2 This finding suggested
    that an objective measure of English proficiency might be more
    powerful than a self-report measure, so that the effect of perceived
    English proficiency was taken up by the SILS vocabulary score when
    they are analyzed simultaneously.

    Table 6
    Moderating effect of culture on the relationship between critical thinking and DSS.

    Steps β t ΔR2 F Change

    1. SILS vocabulary .49 9.60 .24 92.11⁎⁎

    2. SILS vocabulary .47 8.39⁎⁎ .001 .172
    DSS −.01 −.15
    Culture .03 .55

    3. SILS vocabulary .47 8.24⁎⁎ .01 3.17a

    DSS .16 1.50
    Culture .04 .74
    DSS×culture −.19 −1.78a

    3.2.3. Relationship between DSS and WGCTA across cultures
    Table 5 shows the correlations among the target variables in each

    group. Similar correlation pattern among the three test scores were
    observed in both cultural groups, where WGCTA was both positively
    related to SILS vocabulary score and SILS abstraction score, and the
    two SILS scores were moderately positively related to one another.
    This pattern is consistent with those observed in previous research
    (Clifford et al., 2004; Taube, 1997).

    It is worth to note that in the Asian sample, perceived English
    proficiency was significantly related to both SILS vocabulary score and
    cultural adoption, indicating that perceived English proficiency could
    be interpreted both in terms of actual English language abilities and
    behavioral adoption of New Zealand culture.

    Dialectical self-concept was found to be significantly positively
    related to WGCTA score in the Asian sample, whereas its correlation
    with the WGCTA was close to zero in the New Zealand European
    sample. The correlation pattern implicated that the relationship
    between dialectical self-concept and critical thinking vary as a
    function of culture, although dialectical self-concept could not help
    explain the cultural difference in critical thinking performance.
    Moderation analysis using multiple regressions (Baron & Kenny,
    1986) was conducted to reveal the possible cultural difference in how
    dialectical self-concept predicts critical thinking performance. The
    effects of English proficiency (SILS vocabulary scores) was controlled
    for in the analysis because it was found to account for the cultural
    difference in critical thinking performance.

    The results of the regression analyses are reported in Table 6. The
    interaction between culture and dialectical self-concept was margin-
    ally significant (p=.08), suggesting a trend of cultural difference in
    the relationship between DSS and critical thinking skills. It was likely

    2 The point estimate of the specific indirect effect of perceived English proficiency
    was .96, with percentile 95% CI ranged from.11 to 1.94; BC 95% CI ranged from .11 to
    1.95; and BCa 95% CI ranged from .11 to 1.94.

    that if the sample size was larger, the effect would become statistically
    significant because of higher statistical power. The interaction effect is
    depicted in Fig. 1 using the internet version of Modgraph (Jose, 2008).
    It showed that dialectical self-concept was positively related to critical
    thinking skills in the Asian sample, but the relationship between the
    two variables was slightly negative in the New Zealand European
    sample.

    These findings basically replicated the results obtained in Study 1.
    Asian students were again found to perform less well on a critical
    thinking skills assessment than their Western counterparts, which
    provided support to Hypothesis 1. This cultural difference was again
    explained by English proficiency but not dialectical thinking style,
    thereby showing support to Hypothesis 3 but not Hypothesis 2. It is
    also important to note that cultural adoption did not mediate the
    relationship between culture and critical thinking, which ruled out
    the possibility that adoption to Western culture is what explains the
    observed cultural difference. In other words, the result does not
    support the idea that critical thinking favors Western over Asian
    cultural group.

    Interestingly, the relationship between dialectical thinking and
    critical thinking was found to be culturally different. Specifically,
    dialectical thinking was only positively related to critical thinking
    among Asian students but not New Zealand European students,
    suggesting that different cognitive processes might be involved in
    critical thinking between these two samples. Since the current
    analysis revealed that such cultural difference might exist, and
    given that dialectical thinking involves a complex array of cultural
    differences in cognitive tendencies and philosophical traditions (Peng
    & Nisbett, 1999), further research will be needed to unravel the exact
    influence of dialectical thinking style on critical thinking.

    4. Study 2b: critical thinking skills and academic performance

    Study 1 and Study 2a consistently showed that Asian students
    performed less well on two objective measures of critical thinking
    skills than their New Zealand European counterparts. This finding
    might challenge the current enthusiasm on critical thinking in the
    context of international education, because students of different
    cultural backgrounds might show different preferences regarding
    engaging in critical thinking and it may disadvantage some students.
    In such case, the value of promoting critical thinking in university
    education would be undermined as its usefulness varied as a function
    of students’ cultural backgrounds.

    University education in New Zealand showed relatively stronger
    emphasis on critical thinking than that in Asia (Lun, Fischer, & Ward,
    2009). Academic conventions such as writing literature reviews,
    critical reviews, essays, and research proposals are commonplace in
    New Zealand university education, but Asian students often report
    difficulties in fulfilling the requirement of these assessment methods
    because of lack of knowledge about these academic conventions

    Culture was dummy coded as “Asian students=0” and “New Zealand European
    students=1”; DSS was mean centered in the analysis (Aiken & West, 1991).

    a p=.08.
    ⁎⁎ pb .01.

    Fig. 1. Visual depiction of the interaction between DSS and culture on critical thinking in Study 2a.

    612 V.M.-C. Lun et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 20 (2010) 604–616

    (Campbell & Li, 2008). If students’ use of critical thinking is
    encouraged and assessed by these tasks, Asian students might find
    it harder to express their critical thinking as required despite their
    actual ability. Consequently, the relationship between critical thinking
    skills and academic performance might be weaker among Asian
    students. The use of these assessment methods to develop students’
    critical thinking may then be considered inappropriate to Asian
    students.

    In view of these forms of possible cultural influence on the
    relationship between critical thinking and academic performance,
    Hypothesis 4 and Hypothesis 5 as outlined in the Introduction were
    tested in a management course at the university.

    4.1. Participants

    A subgroup of the sample in Study 2a was included in this analysis.
    This sample was drawn from an introductory management course,
    that explicitly focused on critical thinking and the final course grades
    were available for analysis. The subsample comprised 110 New
    Zealand European students and 52 Asian students.

    4.1.1. Course grades in the introductory management course
    The course under investigation was an introductory management

    course which explicitly emphasizes the development of critical
    thinking skills. The course outline stated that the major objective of
    the course was to give an introduction of the trends, issues and
    challenges of the business environment in New Zealand, and students
    were expected to develop and apply their critical thinking in the
    course.

    The assessment components were designed with the aim of
    developing students’ critical thinking through various assignments
    and final examination. These included a journal of the student’s
    personal experience of a business topic (20% of final course grades),
    essays with critical discussion (40% of final course grades), a final
    examination which required critical discussion on issues related to
    business development (30% of final course grades), and satisfactory
    completion of an essay writing and referencing course (10% of final
    course grades). With this explicit emphasis of critical thinking, the
    course offered an interesting avenue for testing the present research
    hypotheses.

    The final course grades of the participants in the introductory
    management course were obtained with approval from the manage-
    ment school as a part of the school’s accreditation project. Ethics
    approval for the accreditation project was granted to the management
    school by the university’s Human Ethics Committee. The grades were
    transformed into a continuous numeric variable based on this scale:
    1=E, 2=D, 3=C, 4=C+, 5=B-, 6=B, 7=B+, 8=A−, 9=A,
    10=A+. According to the grading system of the university, grade C or
    above denotes a pass in the course (i.e., achieving 50% of final course
    grades).

    4.2. Results and Discussion

    4.2.1. Cultural difference in academic performance
    Independent sample t-test revealed that there was a marginally

    significant difference in the academic performance between the two
    groups [t(160)=−1.83, p=.07]: New Zealand European students
    [M=6.41, SD=1.53] performed slightly better than Asian students
    [M=5.92, SD=1.68] in the course which provides initial support to
    Hypothesis 4.

    4.2.2. Relationship between critical thinking and academic performance
    across cultures

    Table 7 shows the correlation among the target variables in the
    two samples. WGCTA was significantly correlated with SILS vocabu-
    lary and SILS abstraction test scores, indicating that critical thinking
    are positively related to one’s English proficiency and general
    intellectual competence. The correlation between course grade and
    the WGCTA score was also positive in both samples. Students’ course
    grades were significantly correlated with SILS abstraction score,
    showing that general intellectual competence is positively associated
    with students’ academic achievement. The correlation between
    course grades and SILS vocabulary score was significant only in the
    Asian sample, but statistical analysis using Fisher z-transformation
    revealed that the difference in the correlation coefficients between
    the two samples was only not significant, z=1.49, p=.14. This
    finding suggested that English proficiency may be a factor in
    predicting students’ academic performance in both samples.

    With regard to Hypothesis 5, moderation analysis using multiple
    regressions (Baron & Kenny, 1986) was used to examine the effects of
    critical thinking skills, cultural adoption, culture, and their interac-
    tions on course grades. SILS vocabulary score, SILS abstraction score,

    Table 7
    Correlation among the variables in the two subsamples in Study 2b.

    Course grade WGCTA SILS vocabulary SILS abstraction DSS Cultural adoption

    WGCTA (.29a) –
    .34⁎⁎

    SILS vocabulary (.45⁎⁎) (.50⁎⁎) –
    .19 .44⁎⁎

    SILS abstraction (.32⁎) (.38⁎) (.38⁎⁎) –
    .30⁎⁎ .30⁎⁎ .28⁎⁎

    DSS (−.09) (.23) (.21) (.37⁎) –
    −.21 .07 .10 .07

    Cultural
    adoption

    (.16) (.13) (.26) (−.13) (−.21) –
    .14 −.14 −.07 −.03 −.12

    Perceived
    English proficiency

    (.20) (.13) (.59⁎⁎) (.26) (.02) (.53⁎⁎)
    .19 .01 .28⁎⁎ .07 −.14 .15

    Numbers in parentheses are correlations in the Asian sample, n=45; Numbers without parentheses are correlations in the New Zealand European sample, n=85; listwise deletion
    of missing data.

    a p=.06.
    ⁎ pb.05.
    ⁎⁎ pb .01.

    613V.M.-C. Lun et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 20 (2010) 604–616

    perceived English proficiency, and gender were entered in the first
    block of the regression analysis to control for their effects to enable a
    clearer picture about the cultural influence on the relationship
    between critical thinking and academic performance. The result of
    the regression analysis is presented in Table 8.

    The first step of the regression analysis showed that course grade
    was significantly predicted by SILS vocabulary score and SILS
    abstraction score, but the effect of gender was not significant. In the
    second step of the regression analysis, it was noted that WGCTA
    significantly predicted course grades, indicating that critical thinking
    was positively related to students’ academic achievement even after
    controlling for the effects of English proficiency and general
    intellectual competence. The significant effect of WGCTA supports
    that the course assessment was positively related to students’ skills in
    practicing critical thinking.

    In the subsequent steps of the regression analysis, the effects of all
    two-way and three-way interaction terms were statistically nonsig-
    nificant, indicating that the relationship between critical thinking and
    academic performance was neither moderated by the cultural back-
    grounds nor the level of cultural adoption of the students. Therefore,
    the results did not support Hypotheses 5. Critical thinking has a
    positive effect on academic performance independent of the cultural
    background of students.

    These results are important in showing that critical thinking skills,
    even after considering the effects of language proficiency and general
    intellectual competence, are predictive of students’ academic perfor-
    mance in a course where the practice of critical thinking is
    emphasized. Though Asian students were found to perform less well
    on critical thinking as measured by the WGCTA, they were not any
    different from their New Zealand European counterparts in using
    critical thinking when it is required in a course. The nonsignificant
    interaction between critical thinking and cultural adoption also
    indicated that acquisition of Western cultural norms did not seem
    to affect the relationship between critical thinking and academic
    performance.

    5. General discussion

    Critical thinking is an important skill for many university students
    (Halpern, 1998; ten Dam & Volman, 2004). We empirically examined
    cultural differences in critical thinking between Asian and Western
    university student samples, addressing an important issue debated in
    the international education literature. We found that New Zealand
    European students performed significantly better than Asian students
    in two objective measures of critical thinking. Investigation of the
    underlying mechanisms revealed that English proficiency, both self-

    report (Study 1) and objectively measured (Study 2a), at least
    partially if not fully account for this observed difference. Although
    dialectical thinking style did not explain the observed cultural
    difference in critical thinking, results in Study 2a suggested that this
    thinking style may be beneficial for Asian students but not New
    Zealand European students in the practice of critical thinking.
    Moreover, despite the observed cultural difference, the relationship
    between critical thinking and academic performance did not vary as a
    function of students’ cultural backgrounds or their level of adoption of
    Western cultural norms (Study 2b).

    The present research confirms with objective assessments the
    cultural differences in critical thinking as observed by ESL teaching
    professionals. While those observations cannot be simply discounted
    as cultural stereotypes of Asian students (e.g., Kumaravadivelu, 2003),
    we should pay attention to the fact that English language proficiency,
    but not dialectical thinking style, explained the difference. In other
    words, the difference in critical thinking appears to be more of a
    linguistic issue rather than a cultural issue. The finding that cultural
    adoption does not mediate the cultural difference in critical thinking
    provided further support to this argument.

    5.1. Dialectical thinking

    Dialectical thinking style did not mediate the observed difference
    in critical thinking skills in the present investigation. In the
    examination of the relationship between DSS and WGCTA scores, it
    was observed that the relationship between the two variables was
    only positive in the Asian sample, but not the New Zealand sample.
    The difference in the direction of correlations might explain why
    dialectical thinking could not mediate the effect of culture on critical
    thinking skills. It should be highlighted, however, that the interaction
    effect was only marginally significant. The result should be considered
    as indicating a possible cultural difference in cognitive processes
    related to critical thinking.

    Dialectical thinking style was a self-report measure in the present
    research. It is possible that individual’s evaluations about various
    dialectical thinking principles might influence the association be-
    tween critical thinking and dialectical thinking in different cultures. In
    a Western culture such as that in New Zealand, the principles involved
    in dialectical thinking might be interpreted as incongruent to the
    formal logical rules which have been commonly endorsed as the
    preferred mode of thinking (Peng & Nisbett, 1999). In this situation, it
    is reasonable to find the principles of dialectical thinking showing a
    negative relationship with critical thinking performance, because the
    endorsement of the principles of dialectical thinking could be seen as
    illogical to critical thinkers. On the other hand, it is possible that the

    Table 8
    Moderating effects of culture and cultural adoption on the relationship between critical
    thinking skills and academic performance.

    Steps β t ΔR2 F Change

    1. SILS vocabulary .22 2.15⁎ .15 5.67⁎⁎⁎

    SILS abstraction .23 2.60⁎⁎

    Perceived English proficiency .06 0.65
    Gender −.07 −0.83

    2. SILS vocabulary .13 1.15 .05 2.59a

    SILS abstraction .20 2.31⁎

    Perceived English proficiency .02 0.23
    Gender −.08 −0.94
    WGCTA .20 2.11⁎

    Cultural adoption .17 1.79b

    Culture −.02 −0.25
    3. SILS vocabulary .17 1.49 .02 1.22

    SILS abstraction .19 2.08⁎

    Perceived English proficiency .02 0.19
    Gender −.09 −1.08
    WGCTA .16 0.98
    Cultural adoption .21 1.35
    Culture −.02 −0.19
    WGCTA X Culture .06 0.39
    Cultural adoption X Culture −.01 −0.05
    Cultural adoption X WGCTA −.15 1.48

    4. SILS vocabulary .17 1.56 .01 2.22
    SILS abstraction .18 2.00⁎

    Perceived English proficiency .02 0.14
    Gender −.08 −0.90
    WGCTA .06 0.33
    Cultural adoption .13 0.78
    Culture .00 0.03
    WGCTA X Culture .11 0.71
    Cultural adoption X Culture .05 0.36
    WGCTA X Cultural adoption −.02 −0.11
    WGCTA X Cultural adoption X Culture .19 1.49

    Culture was dummy coded with “Asian students=0” and “New Zealand European
    students=1”; WGCTA score and cultural adoption were mean centered (Aiken & West,
    1991).

    a p=.06.
    b p= .08.

    ⁎ pb .05.
    ⁎⁎ pb .01.
    ⁎⁎⁎ pb .001.

    614 V.M.-C. Lun et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 20 (2010) 604–616

    endorsement of dialectical thinking was deemed as reflecting an
    individual’s wisdom or intellectual capacities in Asian cultures (see
    Buchtel & Norenzayan, 2008). So, critical thinkers who are skillful in
    the use of different cognitive strategies might prefer the principles of
    dialectical thinking, which resulted in the positive relationship
    between DSS and WGCTA scores.

    The present findings indicated some interesting pattern between
    dialectical thinking style and cognitive tasks such as critical thinking.
    Further examination of the concept of dialectical thinking and
    refinement of its measurement would be beneficial to understand
    the practical implication of the concept to areas such as designing
    appropriate cognitive instruction to students of diverse cultural
    backgrounds.

    5.2. English proficiency

    The present findings confirm Paton’s (2005) suggestion that Asian
    student’s apparent lack of critical thinking is a consequence of the
    need to use English as a second language in academic discourse.
    Without sufficient English proficiency and/or enough confidence in
    using the language, Asian students are discouraged from overtly
    expressing their critical thinking in classrooms even if they want to do
    so.

    According to the CLT (Paas et al., 2003), cognitive load can be
    reduced by forming schemas in the long-term memory that can be

    brought to the working memory for cognitive processing. Increased
    English proficiency resembles formation of schemas about the
    language in the long-term memory so that less cognitive load will
    be created by processing information of the language. It is important
    to note that this principle applies to every student regardless of their
    cultural backgrounds. If a critical thinking task requires information
    processing in a language, students who are proficient in that
    particular language would be able to spare more cognitive capacity
    in the working memory to think critically.

    Critical thinking skills such as verbal reasoning and argument
    analysis demand a certain level of language proficiency. Even the
    mere act of reading a scenario or understanding a problem presented
    in a language requires certain levels of verbal abilities. While it is
    almost impossible to avoid the use of language in critical thinking,
    educators may try to structure assessments in a way that the required
    cognitive load of language proficiency would be kept at a reasonable
    level for all students. For example, overuse of jargon or culture-
    specific slangs or words should be avoided in designing course
    materials or tests that aim at promoting critical thinking among
    students. If the use of such language is deemed necessary, it would be
    important to explain fully and familiarize the students with the
    associated usage.

    The framework proposed by Campbell et al. (2007) for mathe-
    matical instruction for second language learners also provided a
    useful reference of designing critical thinking instruction. One
    element of the framework includes structuring a problem statement
    in a way that students may infer the meanings of certain unfamiliar
    words from the context of the problem. That involves a lot of effort in
    structuring the problem but close coordination between university
    teaching professionals and ESL teachers could help to identify the
    areas of concern and the appropriate solutions.

    5.3. Implication of the practice of critical thinking in international
    education

    It was intriguing to note in Study 2b that academic performance
    was predicted by critical thinking skills after controlling for the effects
    of language and intellectual competence. This finding suggests that
    critical thinking could indeed play a unique role, apart from the more
    commonly emphasized language proficiency and abstract thinking
    ability, in university education. More importantly, our findings
    showed that the relationship between critical thinking and academic
    performance did not seem to be susceptible to the influence of culture.
    We may argue that cultural difference in critical thinking does not
    necessarily constitute a cultural unfairness which undermines the
    value of emphasizing critical thinking in international education
    (Atkinson, 1997). However, it would be important to pay attention to
    the discussed linguistic issues while designing and implementing
    instructional strategies which aim to promote critical thinking among
    students.

    5.4. Limitations and future research

    The present research empirically addressed the claim that Asian
    students lack critical thinking by objectively comparing the critical
    thinking skills between Asian and Western students. However, some
    issues remain to be answered in future research.

    It is important to be aware of the multi-dimensional nature of
    critical thinking. Beside behavioral expressions such as debating or
    questioning ideas (Durkin, 2008), critical thinking also consists of
    different cognitive skills and dispositions (e.g., Ennis, 1987; Facione,
    1990; Halpern, 2003). The present research has been focused on the
    skill aspect of critical thinking and how culture affects this dimension.
    A logical follow-up would be to examine the exact relationship
    between skills, dispositions, and behaviors, and how culture is related
    to the relationships among these variables. Such investigation enables

    615V.M.-C. Lun et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 20 (2010) 604–616

    a more comprehensive understanding of the relationship between
    culture and critical thinking so that proper instructional strategies
    may be designed to cater for the need of promoting critical thinking in
    multicultural classrooms.

    Moreover, the method employed in the present research only
    involved paper-and-pencil form of assessment of critical thinking
    skills. This is only one of the many possible ways for measuring one’s
    critical thinking. After all, critical thinking is not only about test
    performance, it is also the application of different cognitive skills to
    everyday problems in participants’ lives (e.g., Ennis, 1987; Halpern,
    1998). To be able to apply critical thinking to real-life situations is the
    ultimate goal of cultivating critical thinking among students. Research
    on the application of critical thinking to real-life issues, especially
    with reference to the influence of culture, would provide useful
    information about the transfer of critical thinking skills in university
    education.

    Acknowledgements

    We would like to thank the Victoria Management School, Victoria
    University of Wellington, for their generous help with data collection.
    We are also grateful to Prof. Diane Halpern for her permission to
    administer the HCTAES and Pearson Assessment and Information
    Australia and New Zealand for the research sponsorship on the
    WGCTA-SF. This research was supported by grants from the School of
    Psychology, Victoria University of Wellington, and the New Zealand
    International Doctoral Research Scholarship administered by Educa-
    tion New Zealand. Thanks also to Melanie Vauclair, Katja Hanke, and
    Diana Boer for their comments on earlier versions of this paper.

    References

    Aiken, L., & West, S. (1991). Multiple regression. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
    Arlin, P. K. (1984). Arlin test of formal reasoning. East Aurora, NY: Slosson.
    Atkinson, D. (1997). A critical approach to critical thinking in TESOL. TESOL Quarterly, 31

    (1), 71−94.
    Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in

    social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations.
    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173−1182.

    Bernard, R. M., Zhang, D., Abrami, P. C., Sicoly, F., Borokhovski, E., & Surkes, M. A. (2008).
    Exploring the structure of the Watson–Glaser Critical Thinking Appraisal: One scale
    or many subscales? Thinking Skills and Creativity, 3, 15−22.

    Berry, J. W. (1997). Immigration, acculturation, and adaptation. Applied Psychology: An
    International Review, 46(1), 5−34.

    Bowles, K. (2000). The relationship of critical-thinking skills and the clinical-judgment
    skills of baccalaureate nursing students. Journal of Nursing Education, 39(8),
    373−376.

    Buchtel, E. E., & Norenzayan, A. (2008). Which should you use, intuition or logic?
    Cultural differences in injunctive norms about reasoning. Asian Journal of Social
    Psychology, 11, 264−273.

    Campbell, A. E., Adams, V. M., & Davis, G. E. (2007). Cognitive demands and second-
    language learners: A framework for analyzing mathematics instructional contexts.
    Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 9, 3−30.

    Campbell, J., & Li, M. (2008). Asian students’ voices: An empirical study of Asian
    students’ learning experiences at a New Zealand university. Journal of Studies in
    International Education, 12(4), 375−396.

    Clifford, J. S., Boufal, M. M., & Kurtz, J. E. (2004). Personality traits and critical thinking
    skills in college students: Empirical tests of a two-factor theory. Assessment, 11,
    169−176.

    Collins, K. M., & Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2000). Relationship between critical thinking and
    performance in research methodology courses. Paper presented at the Annual
    Meeting of the Mid-South Educational Research Association, Lexington, Kentucky.

    Davidson, B. W. (1998). Comments on Dwight Atkinson’s “A Critical Approach to
    Critical Thinking in TESOL”: A case for critical thinking in the English language
    classroom. TESOL Quarterly, 32(1), 119−123.

    Durkin, K. (2008). The adaptation of East Asian masters students to western norms of
    critical thinking and argumentation in the UK. Intercultural Education, 19(1),
    15−27.

    Ennis, R. H. (1987). A taxonomy of critical thinking dispositions and abilities. In J. Baron,
    & R. Sternberg (Eds.), Teaching thinking skills: Theory and practice (pp. 9−26). New
    York: Freeman.

    Ennis, R. H. (1998). Is critical thinking culturally biased? Teaching Philosophy, 21(1),
    15−33.

    Facione, P. A. (1990). Critical thinking: A statement of expert consensus for purposes of
    educational assessment and instruction. American Philosophical Association (ERIC
    Document Reproduction Services no. ED315 423).

    Friesen, W. (2008). Diverse Auckland: The face of New Zealand in the 21st century?
    Wellington, New Zealand: Asia New Zealand Foundation.

    Gaudet, S., & Clément, R. (2009). Forging an identity as a linguistic minority: Intra- and
    intergroup aspects of language, communication and identity in Western Canada.
    International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 33, 213−227.

    Gieve, S. (1998). Comments on Dwight Atkinson’s “A Critical Approach to Critical
    Thinking in TESOL”: A case for critical thinking in the English language classroom. A
    reader reacts. TESOL Quarterly, 32(1), 123−129.

    Groenvynck, H., Beirens, K., Arends-Tóth, J. V., & Fontaine, J. J. R. (2006). Factorial
    Validity of the Behavioral Acculturation Scale: Evidence for a Bi-Factorial Model.
    Ghent University, Belgium and Tilburg University, The Netherlands.

    Halpern, D. F. (1998). Teaching critical thinking for transfer across domains:
    Dispositions, skills, structure training, and metacognitive monitoring. American
    Psychologist, 53, 449−455.

    Halpern, D. F. (2003). Thought and knowledge: An introduction to critical thinking, 4th
    ed. . Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence.

    Halpern, D. F. (2006). Halpern critical thinking assessment using everyday situations:
    Background and scoring standards. Unpublished manuscript. Claremont, CA:
    Claremont McKenna College.

    Halpern, D. F. (2007). The nature and nurture of critical thinking. In R. J. Sternberg, H. L. I.
    Roediger, & D. F. Halpern (Eds.), Critical thinking in psychology (pp. 1−14). New York:
    Cambridge University Press.

    Hamamura, T., Heine, S. J., & Paulhus, D. L. (2008). Cultural differences in response
    styles: The role of dialectical thinking. Personality and Individual Differences, 44,
    932−942.

    Hau, K. T., Ho, I. T., Lai, Y., Ku, K. Y. L., & Hui, N. H. H. (2008). Chinese students’ critical
    thinking: Validation of the factorial structure of a critical thinking assessment.
    Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association Annual Meeting,
    New York, NY.

    Jose, P. E. (2008). ModGraph-I: A programme to compute cell means for the graphical
    display of moderational analyses: The internet version, Version 2.0. Victoria
    University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand. Retrieved May 28, 2009, from
    http://www.victoria.ac.nz/psyc/staff/paul-jose-files/modgraph/modgraph.php

    Kim, H. S. (2002). We talk, therefore we think? A cultural analysis of the effect of talking
    on thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 828−842.

    Kumaravadivelu, B. (2003). Problematizing cultural stereotypes in TESOL. TESOL
    Quarterly, 37(4), 709−719.

    Lee, K. S., & Carrasquillo, A. (2006). Korean college students in United States:
    Perceptions of professors and students. College Student Journal, 40(2), 442−456.

    Leung, K., & van de Vijver, F. J. R. (2008). Strategies for strengthening causal inferences
    in cross cultural research: The consilience approach. International Journal of Cross
    Cultural Management, 8, 145−169.

    Lun, V. M. C., Fischer, R., & Ward, C. (2009). Teaching critical thinking across cultures: A
    study of the university course syllabi in New Zealand and Hong Kong. In R. Ismail,
    M. E. J. Macapagal, N. M. Noor, J. Takai, & T. Hur (Eds.), Global issues and challenges in
    a changed world: Psychological, cultural and group relationships (pp. 131−147). Kota
    Kinabalu, Malaysia: Center for Research and Innovation, Universiti Malaysia Sabah.

    MacKinnon, D. P., Fairchild, A. J., & Fritz, M. S. (2007). Mediation analysis. Annual Review
    of Psychology, 58, 593−614.

    MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A
    comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects.
    Psychological Methods, 7(1), 83−104.

    Matsumoto, D., & Yoo, S. H. (2006). Toward a new generation of cross-cultural research.
    Perspectives in Psychological Science, 1, 234−250.

    New Zealand Ministry of Education. (2008). International enrolments in New Zealand.
    Retrieved October 24, 2008, from http://www.educationcounts.govt.nz/publica-
    tions/international/15260/28332/24707.

    Nickerson, R. S., Perkins, D., & Smith, E. E. (1985). Teaching thinking. Hillsdale, NJ:
    Erlbaum Associates.

    Norenzayan, A., Smith, E. E., Kim, B. J., & Nisbett, R. E. (2002). Cultural preferences for
    formal versus intuitive reasoning. Cognitive Science, 26, 653−684.

    Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought:
    Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108(2), 291−310.

    OECD. (2009). Education at a Glance 2009: OECD Indicators. Retrieved February 25,
    2010, from http://www.oecd.org/dataoecd/41/25/43636332

    Paas, F., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (2003). Cognitive load theory and instructional design:
    Recent developments. Educational Psychologist, 38, 63−71.

    Pascarella, E. T., & Terenzini, P. T. (2005). How college affects students: A third decade of
    research. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Paton, M. (2005). Is critical analysis foreign to Chinese students? In E. Manalo, & G.
    Wong-Toi (Eds.), Communication skills in university education: The international
    dimension (pp. 1−11). Auckland, New Zealand: Pearson Education New Zealand.

    Paulson, M. J., & Lin, T. (1970). Predicting WAIS IQ from Shipley–Hartford scores. Journal
    of Clinical Psychology, 22, 294−296.

    Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction.
    American Psychologist, 54, 741−754.

    Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (2000). Dialectical responses to questions about dialectical
    thinking. American Psychologist, 55, 1067−1068.

    Pithers, R. T., & Soden, R. (2000). Critical thinking in education: A review. Educational
    Research, 42(3), 237−249.

    Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect
    effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, &
    Computers, 36, 717−731.

    Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing
    and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research
    Methods, 40, 879−891.

    616 V.M.-C. Lun et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 20 (2010) 604–616

    Renaud, R. D., & Murray, H. G. (2008). A comparison of a subject-specific and a general
    measure of critical thinking. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 3, 85−93.

    Robertson, M., Line, M., Jones, S., & Thomas, S. (2000). International students, learning
    environments and perceptions: A case study using the Delphi technique. Higher
    Education Research & Development, 19(1), 89−102.

    Shin, K. R. (1998). Critical thinking ability and clinical decision-making skills among
    senior nursing students in associate and baccalaureate. Journal of Advanced Nursing,
    27, 414−418.

    Shipley, W. C. (1940). A self-administering scale for measuring intellectual impairment
    and deterioration. Journal of Psychology, 9, 371−377.

    Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic intervals for indirect effects in structural equations
    models. In S. Leinhart (Ed.), Sociological methodology 1982 (pp. 290−312). San
    Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Spector, P. E., Schneider, J. R., Vance, C. A., & Hazlett, S. A. (2000). The relation of
    cognitive ability and personality traits to assessment center performance. Journal of
    Applied Social Psychology, 30(7), 1474−1491.

    Spencer-Rodgers, J., Peng, K., Wang, L., & Hou, Y. (2004). Dialectical self-esteem and
    East–West differences in psychological wellbeing. Personality and Social Psychology
    Bulletin, 30(1), 1416−1432.

    Spencer-Rodgers, J., Srivastava, S., & Peng, K. (2001). The dialectical self scale.
    Unpublished data, as cited in Spencer-Rodgers, J., Peng, K., Wang, L., & Hou, Y.
    (2004). Dialectical self-esteem and East–West differences in psychological well-
    being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1416–1432.

    Taube, K. T. (1997). Critical thinking ability and disposition as factors of performance on
    a written critical thinking test. Journal of General Education, 46, 129−164.

    Takano, Y., & Noda, A. (1993). A temporary decline of thinking ability during foreign
    language processing. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 24(4), 445−462.

    Ten Dam, G., & Volman, M. (2004). Critical thinking as a citizenship competence:
    Teaching strategies. Learning and Instruction, 14, 359−379.

    Tweed, R. G., & Lehman, D. R. (2002). Learning considered within a cultural context:
    Confucian and Socratic approaches. American Psychologist, 57(2), 89−99.

    Ward, C. (1996). Acculturation. In D. Landis, & R. S. Bhagat (Eds.), Handbook of
    intercultural training (pp. 124−147)., 2 ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Watkins, D. A., & Biggs, J. B. (2001). The Paradox of the Chinese Learner and Beyond. In
    D. A. Watkins, & J. B. Biggs (Eds.), Teaching the Chinese learner: Psychological and
    pedagogical perspectives (pp. 3−23). Hong Kong: Comparative Education Research
    Centre, The University of Hong Kong.

    Watson, G., & Glaser, E. M. (1980). Watson–Glaser Critical Thinking Appraisal: Forms A
    and B. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Watson, G., & Glaser, E. M. (1994). Watson–Glaser Critical Thinking Appraisal, Form S
    manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Watson, G., & Glaser, E. M. (2006). Watson–Glaser Critical Thinking Appraisal-Short
    Form Manual. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.

    Wechsler, D. (1981). Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. New
    York: Psychological Corporation.

    Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition. San Antonio, TX:
    Psychological Corporation.

    Zachary, R. A. (1991). Shipley Institute of Living Scale Revised Manual. Los Angeles, CA:
    Western Psychological Services.

    Zachary, R. A., Paulson, M. J., & Gorsuch, R. (1985). Estimating WAIS IQ from the Shipley
    Institute of Living Scale using continuously adjusted age norms. Journal of Clinical
    Psychology, 41(6), 820−831.

    • Exploring cultural differences in critical thinking: Is it about my thinking style or the language I speak?
    • Introduction
      Do Asian students think less critically?
      Critical thinking — a multi-dimensional construct
      Focus of the present research
      Cultural differences in critical thinking
      Dialectical thinking
      English proficiency
      Possible consequence of cultural differences in critical thinking
      Overview of the present research
      Study 1: pilot study with the HCTAES
      Method
      Participants
      Materials
      Halpern Critical Thinking Assessment using Everyday Situations (HCTAES)
      Dialectical Self Scale (DSS)
      Perceived proficiency in reading and writing English (English)
      Analytical strategy
      Results and discussion
      Cultural differences in critical thinking skills and other self-report measures
      Mediation between culture and critical thinking skills

      Study 2a: assessment with the WGCTA-SF
      Method
      Participants
      Materials
      Watson–Glaser Critical Thinking Appraisal Short Form (WGCTA-SF)
      Shipley Institute of Living Scale (SILS)
      Dialectical Self Scale (DSS)
      Perceived English proficiency (English)
      Cultural adoption of behavioral norms in New Zealand

      Results and discussion
      Cultural differences in WGCTA, SILS scores, and other self-report measures
      Mediation between culture and critical thinking skills
      Relationship between DSS and WGCTA across cultures

      Study 2b: critical thinking skills and academic performance
      Participants
      Course grades in the introductory management course
      Results and Discussion
      Cultural difference in academic performance
      Relationship between critical thinking and academic performance across cultures
      General discussion
      Dialectical thinking
      English proficiency
      Implication of the practice of critical thinking in international education
      Limitations and future research
      Acknowledgements
      References

    Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

    Thinking Skills and Creativity

    journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/tsc

    A meta-analysis on critical thinking and community college student
    achievement☆

    Carlton J. Fonga,⁎, Yughi Kima, Coreen W. Davisa, Theresa Hoanga, Young Won Kimb

    a Department of Curriculum and Instruction, Texas State University, USA
    b Department of Educational Psychology, University of Texas at Austin, USA

    A R T I C L E I N F O

    Keywords:
    Critical thinking
    Academic achievement
    Community college
    Two-year college
    Meta-analysis

    A B S T R A C T

    The purpose of this meta-analysis was to examine the relationship between student levels of
    critical thinking (as established via critical thinking tests) and community college student suc-
    cess. We conducted a meta-analysis to synthesize the extant literature on critical thinking and
    community college student success. After systematically searching the relevant literature through
    electronic databases using an array of search terms, we screened studies and reviewed them for
    inclusion. Included studies were reliably coded using a protocol to extract correlational effect size
    data and study characteristics. These effect sizes were aggregated meta-analytically. From a total
    of 23 studies (27 samples, N =

    82

    33), we found that critical thinking was moderately related to
    community college student success. The relationship between student achievement and levels of
    critical thinking (as skills or dispositions) was constant for both nursing and non-nursing students
    as well as for grades and individual test outcomes; however, students’ levels of critical thinking
    was more strongly associated with longer-term outcomes compared with those at the shorter
    term. Meta-regression results indicated some evidence that effects were weaker for male and
    minority community college students. Implications for this study include the importance of
    cultivating critical thinking for all community college students and the exploration of under-
    studied areas for future research.

    1. Introduction

    Critical thinking is arguably one of the most valued learning goals in postsecondary education (Gellin, 2003; Pithers & Soden,
    2000; Wilson & Wagner, 19

    81

    ). In an expanding world of information, there is growing need for individuals to process data, evaluate
    ideas, and reason through arguments (Pascarella & Terenzini, 2005). Critical thinking is particularly critical for community college
    students engaged in vocational training or preparing for transfer to four-year institutions. Community college enrollment has rapidly
    expanded over the last century, affording greater participation in higher education, especially for individuals with limited oppor-
    tunities (Goldrick-Rab, 2010; Stevens & Kirst, 2015; Zientek, Yetkiner Ozel, Fong, & Griffin, 2013). In fact, recent statistics indicated
    that nearly 40% of students in higher education are enrolled in community colleges (Shapiro, Dundar, Yuan, Harrell, & Wakhungu,
    2014). However, despite the recent rise of community colleges, failure to match students to credentials is a persistent issue. Research
    has consistently shown low degree completion rates (Fike & Fike, 2008); in fact, only one third of community college students earned
    a credential within six years (Calcagno, Bailey, Jenkins, Kienzl, & Leinbach, 2006). Moreover, some reports indicated that almost half

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2017.06.002
    Received 20 October 2016; Received in revised form 10 April 2017; Accepted 6 June 2017

    ☆ Parts of this paper were presented at the annual meeting of the Council for the Study of Community Colleges. This project has been funded through an American
    Psychological Association Educational Psychology Early Career Grant.
    ⁎ Corresponding author at: Department of Curriculum and Instruction, Texas State University, San Marcos, TX,

    78

    666, USA.
    E-mail address: carltonfong@txstate.edu (C.J. Fong).

    Thinking Skills and Creativity 26 (2017) 71–

    83

    Available online 15 June 2017
    1871-1871/ © 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

    MARK

    http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/18711871

    http://www.elsevier.com/locate/tsc

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2017.06.002

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2017.06.002

    mailto:carltonfong@txstate.edu

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2017.06.002

    http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1016/j.tsc.2017.06.002&domain=pdf

    of community college students dropped out within their first year (Xu & Jaggars, 2011).
    The preponderance of the literature examining factors affecting community college students’ academic success and persistence

    has tended to concentrate on variables such as first-generation status, socioeconomic status, and prior school achievement. Although
    it is vital to identify such background factors, the use of prescriptive measures which assess students’ cognitive, motivational, and
    behavioral variables affecting access, success, and retention cannot be overlooked (Fong et al., 2017). By identifying variables which
    can be enhanced through educational interventions, such as students’ critical thinking skills, educators and practitioners can design
    and implement interventions to help students improve in these areas.

    1.1. Critical thinking

    Critical thinking is an elusive construct to define and understand, partly due to a variety of definitions in the field (Moore, 2013).
    For our study, a working definition of critical thinking is “purposeful, self-regulatory judgment that results in interpretation, analysis,
    evaluation, and inference, as well as explanations of the considerations which that judgment is based”. Critical thinking has been
    conceived of in two ways: skills and disposition. Critical thinking skills involve a set of capacities including interpreting, predicting,
    analyzing, and evaluating (Abrami et al., 2008). On the other hand, dispositions refer to an individual’s proclivities and personality
    characteristics related with aspects of critical thinking such as curiosity, inquisitiveness, open-mindedness, and prudence in decision-
    making (Facione, Facione, & Sanchez, 1994). Some of the most common measurements of critical thinking skills and/or dispositions
    are the Watson-Glaser Critical Thinking Appraisal tool (Watson & Glaser, 1964), Cornell Critical Thinking Test (Ennis,
    Millman, & Tomko, 1985), Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (Pintrich, Smith, Garcia, & McKeachie, 1993), and Cali-
    fornia Critical Thinking Disposition Inventory (Facione, Facione, & Giancarlo, 2001).

    Critical thinking has been used as an appropriate framework for adult education (Garrison, 1992). Applying critical thinking to
    adult learners, Brookfield (1987) outlined five phases: a triggering event, appraisal of the situation, exploration and explanation,
    development of alternatives, and integration of perspectives. Particularly important for adult learners who prefer self-directed
    learning, critical thinking places on the learner the responsibility of making sense of a situation or integrating new ideas with
    previous knowledge. In a variety of educational settings, there is evidence that supports how critical thinking increases learning
    outcomes (Facione, 2009; Halpern, 1998; Heijltjes, van Gog, Leppink, & Paas, 2014). Evidence from a prior meta-analytic review has
    illustrated the positive impact of the college experience on developing critical thinkers (Huber & Kuncel, 2016; McMillan, 1987).
    Moreover, another recent meta-analysis has documented effective strategies that target critical thinking. Despite recent syntheses on
    critical thinking, the context of the community college and the role of critical thinking in student achievement have yet to be explored
    meta-analytically.

    1.2. Critical thinking and community college students

    In the community college setting overall, critical thinking is considered an ideal learning outcome (Bers, McGowan, & Rubin,
    1996). However, there is inconsistent evidence regarding the boon of critical thinking for community college students. For example,
    Thompson (2009) examined two groups of two-year college students: a successful group (grades of A/B) and an unsuccessful group
    (grades of C or worse). There were significantly different levels of critical thinking disposition between the two groups with the
    successful group reporting high levels of critical thinking. Alternatively, in a distance-learning class, Puzziferro (2006) also compared
    successful and unsuccessful students on their course grades and critical thinking but found no significant differences. Similarly,
    Singleton-Williams (2009) found a non-significant correlation between students’ levels of critical thinking and final course grades in
    an introductory computer application class. Whereas some studies may show negative effects (e.g., Foust, 2005), some studies show
    no effects or positive relations with achievement (e.g., Thompson, 2006). Due to the discrepancies in the literature as well as the
    prevalence of critical thinking as a proposed goal of the community college experience, a research synthesis to aggregate previous
    findings is timely and important.

    One subpopulation among community college students in which critical thinking is particularly salient is nursing students.
    Because of the rapidly changing healthcare environment and the need to quickly process and interpret multiple sources of in-
    formation, critical thinking is an indisputable characteristic nursing educators desire to cultivate within their students
    (Scheffer & Rubenfeld, 2000). Moreover, because community colleges produce a significant amount of nurses in our society
    (Jacobs & Dougherty, 2006), cultivating critical thinking at the community college level is becoming increasingly important.

    1.3. Factors related to critical thinking and college student achievement

    A number of factors may influence the relationship between students’ levels of critical thinking and their academic achievement.
    Student background characteristics, such as demographic information like gender and ethnicity, have been examined in the literature
    on critical thinking; however, the evidence is mixed. Regarding gender, some studies have reported no differences on critical thinking
    measures, differences favoring women, or differences favoring men (Giancarlo & Facione, 2001; King, Wood, & Mines, 1990). Re-
    garding ethnicity, Terenzini, Springer, Pascarella, and Nora (1995) found that ethnicity (White vs. non-White) was not a significant
    predictor of students’ levels of critical thinking skills. Furthermore, a comparison between traditional college students and first-
    generation students, who tend to consist of historically underrepresented minorities, yielded no significant differences in critical
    thinking skills (Terenzini, Springer, Yaeger, Pascarella, & Nora, 1996). Although ethnicity may not play a direct role in students’
    levels of critical thinking skills, there is compelling evidence regarding the influence of diversity and diversity-related experiences

    C.J. Fong et al. Thinking Skills and Creativity 26 (2017) 71–83

    72

    that enhance students’ cognitive and intellectual development (Mayhew et al., 2016). For example, in a meta-analysis by Bowman
    (2010), the strongest positive associations between diversity experiences and college student cognitive development were found
    when collegians had interpersonal, interracial experiences. To further clarify how gender, ethnicity, and diversity moderate the
    relation between students’ levels of critical thinking and their achievement, we planned moderator tests to assess these student
    characteristics.

    Another important college student characteristic to consider is students’ academic discipline or major. Similarly, the literature
    regarding the influence of academic major on critical thinking is mixed (Mayhew et al., 2016). A large number of studies have
    demonstrated that students in the physical sciences, life sciences, engineering, and math generally had significantly higher critical
    thinking scores than students in the humanities and social sciences (Arum & Roksa, 2011; Brint, Cantwell, & Saxena, 2012; King et al.,
    1990). On the other hand, research has indicated no significant differences among majors (Li, Long, & Simpson, 1999). In particular,
    Pike, Kuh, and McCormick (2011) found disciplinary differences on higher order thinking among college freshmen, yet these dif-
    ferences were not apparent among college seniors. Categorizing majors into practice disciplines (i.e., nursing, education, business)
    and nonpractice disciplines (i.e., English, history, psychology), Walsh and Hardy (1999) found that in general, students in non-
    practice disciplines had more favorable levels of critical thinking dispositions. In the community college literature, critical thinking
    has been frequently studied in the nursing field. Therefore, the current meta-analysis will measure disciplinary differences as a
    moderator, namely between nursing fields and non-nursing fields, mirroring the contrast of practice discipline versus nonpractice
    discipline, used by Walsh and Hardy (1999).

    1.4. Research questions

    Our meta-analytic study was guided by two research questions: 1) What is the relationship (direction and magnitude) between
    critical thinking and community college student success in the existing literature? 2) What factors explain variation in the re-
    lationship between critical thinking and community college student success?

    2. Method

    Research syntheses aggregate the findings of studies that address the same research question. Bowman (2012) encouraged the use
    of quantitative research synthesis, or meta-analysis, in higher education research. Despite the rise of meta-analytic research in
    education, use of this methodology in the community college field is scant. In the following sections, we briefly outline the meth-
    odological and analytic approaches used in our meta-analysis, which is closely aligned with the methods from Cooper, Hedges, and
    Valentine (2009) and Cooper (2010).

    2.1. Literature search and inclusion criteria

    The first step in conducting the research synthesis was to exhaustively search the literature for studies on critical thinking and
    community college success. In order to do so, we used an array of search strategies to comprehensively uncover relevant studies (see
    Table 1). We opted to choose a broad set of keyword terms because researchers may not explicitly mention or describe critical
    thinking. Retrieved studies must have included at least one search term in the following three domains: predictors related to critical
    thinking, populations related to community college, and outcomes related to achievement and persistence. We searched the following
    electronic databases: ERIC, PsycINFO, and Proquest Dissertation and Theses Full Text. The literature search did not have an explicit start
    date and ended in January 2015. In addition, we conducted a number of hand searches in the most prominent community college
    journals, namely, Community College Journal of Research and Practice and Community College Review.

    After all citations were retrieved, we screened titles and abstracts of each of the citations for potential relevance to the topic. The
    full-text documents of the resulting pool of studies were located and screened using the following inclusion criteria (Fig. 1 depicts the
    search retrieval and screen flow chart). Within each full-text document, we also conducted an ancestry search, examining the

    Table 1
    Search Strategy.

    Domain Keywords

    Predictor “critical thinking” OR “creative thinking” OR “study habits” OR “study skills” OR “problem solving” OR “conflict resolution” OR “decision
    making” OR “psychosocial factors” OR “time management” OR adjustment OR “communication skills” OR adaptability OR “career development”
    OR “vocational maturity” OR self-efficacy OR “student attitudes” OR planning OR conscientiousness OR “work attitudes” OR involvement OR
    determin* OR factor* OR variable* OR parameter* OR reason* OR caus* OR correlat* R antecedent* OR predictor*

    Population “community college” OR “two year college” OR “junior college” OR “two-year college” OR undergraduate OR freshman OR sophomore OR
    “junior student” OR “senior student” OR “transfer student” OR “vocational school” OR “vocational college” OR “technical school” OR “technical
    college” OR “associate’s degree” OR “city college”

    Outcome “academic achievement” OR “academic performance” OR grade* OR scholastic OR grade point average OR GPA OR degree OR “college
    performance” OR “college achievement” OR mark OR graduation OR completion OR attainment OR “retention” OR “school holding power” OR
    “academic persistence” OR attendance OR dropout OR “dropping out” OR “enrollment management” OR “student attrition” OR truancy OR
    withdrawal

    Note. Asterisks denote truncation, and quotation marks surround a phrase. Each domain of keywords is linked by the AND Boolean.

    C.J. Fong et al. Thinking Skills and Creativity 26 (2017) 71–83

    73

    reference lists to uncover any other potentially relevant studies.
    To be included in our meta-analysis, a study must have had the following components: a) community college sample, b) critical

    thinking variable, c) student success outcome, and (d) effect size information to estimate a correlational relationship between critical
    thinking and a success outcome. For the sample, students had to be enrolled in a community college, two-year vocational school, or
    two-year technical school. For the critical thinking variable, any measure of critical thinking skills or dispositions must have been
    used. Measurements of critical thinking can be a formal assessment of critical thinking skills (i.e., Watson-Glaser Critical Thinking
    Assessment) or a self-reported measure regarding the efficacy for or use of critical thinking skills as well as dispositions regarding
    critical thinking.

    For the outcome criteria, we included studies with persistence- and achievement-related outcomes. Achievement-related out-
    comes consisted of course grades, GPA, and test scores such as standardized achievement measures (Persistence outcomes were not
    reported in relation to critical thinking in our pool of included studies because we only found one study with persistence outcomes:
    Eason, 1986). For the final criterion, effect size information was required for a study to be included, which mainly consisted of
    Pearson r correlations.

    2.2. Information retrieval and coding

    Once full-texts were deemed to meet our inclusion criteria, a team of five coders retrieved information on various characteristics
    for each of the research reports. Because every study report differs in how much detail it provides, we inferred a number of codes
    when necessary by using pre-established definitions to code ambiguous characteristics. The codes were categorized into four do-
    mains: research report, sample, predictor variable, and outcome variable.

    Research report characteristics. First, we coded characteristics regarding the report, such as the author name, year of the
    report, and type of report. We were mainly interested in the type of report to ascertain whether a work was published in a peer-
    reviewed journal or not. This allowed us to assess publication bias, an important feature when conducting a meta-analysis (Polanin,
    Tanner-Smith, & Hennessy, 2016).

    Sample characteristics. Second, we coded for sample characteristics in each report. We coded for the community college setting
    as well as demographic characteristics such as age, gender, ethnicity, and socioeconomic status. We also coded for educational
    characteristics related to the sample, including program/major, full-time status, and hours worked per week.

    Predictor characteristics. Third, we coded aspects of the predictor variable such as the type of psychosocial variable and study
    authors’ descriptions of the predictor. We also noted if there was a scale or instrument name, reliability of the measure, and domain.
    Two aspects of interest were whether the critical thinking measure described skills or a disposition and whether the measure was self-
    reported (i.e., self-efficacy for or use of skills) versus a formal assessment of their critical thinking abilities.

    Outcome characteristics. Fourth, we coded characteristics of the outcome. Namely, we were interested in student success
    outcomes, which comprise of either retention in community college, degree attainment, and course completion or achievement-
    related outcomes such as grades, GPA, or tests. We also coded for the duration of the persistence measure (one semester or beyond
    one semester) to compare the shorter-term impacts with longer-term ones.

    Coder reliability. All reports were coded independently by five trained coders. The coders had experience in meta-analytic
    coding and were extensively trained on each code using the previously mentioned coding frame. For a reliability check, we compared
    all pairs of codes for each study between two coders. We calculated a reliability measure by dividing the number of matched codes by
    the total number of codes. Disagreements were noted and resolved by another coder. Coder interrater reliability was high with an

    Fig. 1. Search retrieval and screening flow diagram.

    C.J. Fong et al. Thinking Skills and Creativity 26 (2017) 71–83

    74

    agreement rate of 96%.

    2.3. Effect size calculation and data integration

    Effect sizes were computed as Pearson’s r. We converted the correlations to Fisher’s z, conducted our analyses, then converted
    back to r. When possible, we extracted correlations and sample sizes from correlation tables or text in the reports. If data were only
    available from means, standard deviations, and sample sizes of two groups (i.e., high achievers and low achievers), we estimated a
    correlation. When this information was not reported in a study, corresponding inference test statistics (e.g., t-statistic, F-statistic, p-
    values, chi-squared test) were used to derive an effect size. If statistical significance was denoted yet both raw data and inferential test
    statistics were unavailable, a conservative effect size was derived with an assumed p-value of 0.05.

    We used a shifting-unit-of-analysis approach (Cooper, 2010) in order to deal with the issue of determining what constitutes an
    independent estimate of effect. This approach involves coding as many effect sizes from each study that exist as a result of variations
    within the study. Then, we averaged effects appropriately in analyses to prevent violating the assumption of independence at various
    data points.

    Accounting for sample size, we used weighted procedures to calculate average effect sizes across all comparisons. The weights
    were calculated by multiplying each independent effect size by the inverse of its variance and then the sum of these products is then
    divided by the sum of the inverses (see Cooper et al., 2009). We also calculated 95% confidence intervals (CI) for each effect size.
    Synthesis procedures were conducted on Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2005).

    Because of inherent measurement error in instruments (in our case, critical thinking measures), we also calculated unattenuated
    correlation effect sizes. Using psychometric methods recommended by Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, and Rothstein (2009), we
    converted observed or attenuated correlations to adjusted or unattenuated correlations, using the reliability coefficient of each
    critical thinking measure. We then conducted a second overall meta-analysis using the adjusted effect sizes. Using estimated total
    variance of the observed effect estimates and the estimated variance of the true effects, we calculated a proportion of variance in the
    observed or attenuated effects explained by artifacts of measurement error or sampling error in the critical thinking variable.

    In a research synthesis, there is a theoretical possibility of not obtaining all studies that have investigated the relationship between
    critical thinking and community college student success, either due to failure on the part of the meta-analyst to retrieve all relevant
    reports or censoring on the part of individual authors. Therefore, we employed Duval and Tweedie’s (2000) trim-and-fill procedure to
    assess whether the effect size distribution differed from normally distributed estimates. Trim-and-fill methods impute missing values
    that would be present to achieve an approximately normal distribution of effect sizes. Thus, we provided corrected effect sizes when
    effect sizes were deemed missing on the right or left size of the distribution to indicate the impact of data censoring on the effect size
    distribution.

    Analyses were conducted using both random error (RE) assumptions and fixed error (FE) assumptions. Random error assumptions
    account for variance at the study level (Hedges & Vevea, 1998), which is assumed to be an additional source of random variation,
    whereas a fixed of error does not but rather, assumes that differences among participants are the sole source of error. However,
    neither model is perfect. Fixed error assumptions do not take into consideration how study-level features contribute random error to a
    set of correlational data. Overton (1998) cautioned that random effects can overestimate error variance and produce overly con-
    servative confidence intervals when assumptions are violated. Because both models can be flawed, we provide our results using both
    FE and RE as a way of sensitivity analysis as recommended by Greenhouse and Iyengar (1994). If there are discrepancies between
    both sets of error, i.e., a significant result using FE and a nonsignificant result using RE, then we will interpret our findings presuming
    that a significant result using both models of error is considered the most robust (Cooper, 2010). This approach follows other meta-
    analytic studies in the educational and psychological literature (Dent & Koenka, 2016; Patall, Cooper, & Robinson, 2008).

    Moderator analyses. Effect sizes may vary even if they estimate the same underlying population value. Therefore, homogeneity
    analyses were necessary to examine whether sampling error alone accounted for this variance compared to the observed variance
    caused by study features. We tested homogeneity of the observed set of effect sizes using a within-class goodness-of-fit statistic (Qw).
    A significant Qw statistic suggests that sampling variation alone could not adequately explain the variability in the effect size esti-
    mation, so moderators should then be examined (Cooper et al., 2009). Similarly, homogeneity analyses can be used to determine
    whether multiple groups of average effect sizes vary more than predicted by sampling error. In this case, statistical differences among
    different categories of studies were tested by computing the between-class goodness-of-fit statistic, Qb. A significant Qb statistic
    indicates that average effect sizes vary between categories of the moderator variables more than predicted by sampling error alone.
    For continuous moderators, we used meta-regression to assess the moderation of continuous variables on the correlation between
    critical thinking and student success.

    3. Results

    After employing our search for relevant studies, we uncovered 11,832 unique reports. Upon reviewing titles and abstracts, we
    selected 697 studies for full-text retrieval in order to evaluate them using the inclusion criteria. Our final pool included 27 samples (k)
    from 23 studies spanning the years 19

    76

    –2014. The total sample size of participants across all studies was 8233 (N) with an average
    sample size of 316.7 per study. After using Grubbs’ test to identify outliers within that set of effect sizes, no outliers were detected.

    C.J. Fong et al. Thinking Skills and Creativity 26 (2017) 71–83

    75

    T
    ab

    le
    2

    C
    h
    ar
    ac
    te
    ri
    st
    ic
    s
    of

    In
    cl
    u
    d
    ed

    St
    u
    d
    ie
    s.

    A
    u

    th
    or

    (Y
    ea
    r)

    P
    u
    bl
    ic
    at
    io
    n
    T
    yp

    e
    Sa

    m
    p
    le

    Si
    ze

    (F
    em

    al
    e%

    )
    M
    in
    or
    it
    y
    (%

    )
    M
    aj
    or
    /
    P
    ro
    gr
    am

    C
    T
    T
    yp

    e
    C
    T
    M
    ea
    su
    re

    (r
    el
    ia
    bi
    li
    ty
    )

    O
    u
    tc
    om

    e
    D
    u
    ra
    ti
    on

    r
    r

    ru

    B
    ac
    h
    m
    an

    (1
    9
    9
    8
    )

    D
    is
    se
    rt
    at
    io
    n

    7
    8
    (9
    2
    %
    )

    N
    R

    N
    u
    rs
    in
    g

    D
    C
    C
    T
    D
    I
    (0
    .9
    0
    )

    G
    P
    A
    /G

    ra
    d
    es

    >

    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r

    0
    .2
    6

    0
    .2
    7

    0
    .2
    9

    S
    C
    C
    T
    ST

    (0
    .7
    8
    )

    0
    .2
    8

    C
    or
    n
    el
    iu
    s
    (2
    0
    1
    1
    )

    D
    is
    se
    rt
    at
    io
    n

    1
    7
    2
    (9
    3
    %
    )

    6
    %

    N
    u
    rs
    in
    g

    S
    C
    T
    A

    (

    0
    .6
    9

    *)

    G
    P
    A
    /G
    ra
    d
    es

    >
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r

    0
    .2
    2

    0
    .2
    2
    0
    .2
    6

    C
    ri
    n
    er

    (1
    9
    9
    2
    )

    D
    is
    se
    rt
    at
    io
    n

    4
    1
    (7
    3
    %
    )

    N
    R

    N
    on

    -n
    u
    rs
    in
    g

    S
    N
    JT

    R
    S
    (0
    .7
    0
    *)

    G
    P
    A
    /G
    ra
    d
    es
    >
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r

    0
    .4
    2

    0
    .4
    2

    0
    .5
    0

    E
    as
    on

    (1
    9
    8
    6
    )

    D
    is
    se
    rt
    at
    io
    n

    1
    8
    7
    (6
    2
    %
    )

    N
    R
    N
    on
    -n
    u
    rs
    in
    g

    S
    W
    G
    C
    T
    A

    (0
    .7
    7
    )

    G
    P
    A
    /G
    ra
    d
    es
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r

    0
    .0
    5

    0
    .0
    6

    0
    .0
    0

    T
    es
    t
    sc
    or
    e

    >
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r

    0
    .1
    6

    Fo
    u
    st

    (2
    0
    0
    8
    )

    D
    is
    se
    rt
    at
    io
    n

    2
    1
    (6
    2
    %
    )

    2

    0
    %

    N
    on
    -n
    u
    rs
    in
    g

    S

    M
    SL

    Q
    (0
    .1
    6
    )

    G
    P
    A
    /G
    ra
    d
    es
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r


    0
    .3
    5


    0
    .3
    5


    0
    .8
    8

    G
    ay

    th
    w
    ai
    te

    (2
    0
    0
    6
    )

    D
    is
    se
    rt
    at
    io
    n

    5
    7
    (

    6
    7
    %

    )

    2
    3
    %

    N
    on
    -n
    u
    rs
    in
    g
    S
    M
    SL

    Q

    (0
    .8
    0
    )

    G
    P
    A
    /G
    ra
    d
    es
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r

    0
    .1
    4

    0
    .1
    4

    0
    .1
    5

    G
    u
    st
    er

    an
    d
    B
    at
    t
    (1
    9
    8
    9
    )

    A
    rt
    ic
    le

    5
    0
    (5
    8
    %
    )

    N
    R
    N
    on
    -n
    u
    rs
    in
    g
    S
    W
    G
    C
    T
    A

    (0
    .8
    3
    )

    T
    es
    t
    sc
    or
    e
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r
    0
    .1
    4
    0
    .1
    4
    0
    .1
    5

    H
    ea
    rr
    on

    (1
    9
    9
    1
    )

    D
    is
    se
    rt
    at
    io
    n

    2
    9
    4
    (7
    7
    %
    )

    1
    6
    %

    N
    on
    -n
    u
    rs
    in
    g

    S
    T
    O
    LT

    (0
    .8
    3
    )
    G
    P
    A
    /G
    ra
    d
    es
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r

    0
    .4
    7

    0
    .4
    7

    0
    .5
    1

    H
    u
    ro
    v
    (1
    9
    8
    7
    )

    R
    ep

    or
    t

    1
    1
    8
    (8
    0
    %
    )

    1
    0
    %

    N
    u
    rs
    in
    g
    S
    W
    G
    C
    T
    A
    (0
    .8
    0
    )
    G
    P
    A
    /G
    ra
    d
    es
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r

    0
    .2
    5

    0
    .2
    5
    0
    .2
    8

    K
    u
    zn

    ar
    (2
    0
    0
    9
    )

    D
    is
    se
    rt
    at
    io
    n

    1
    3
    6
    (5
    3
    %
    )

    1
    0
    %
    N
    u
    rs
    in
    g
    S
    M
    SL

    Q
    (0
    .7
    9
    )

    T
    es
    t
    sc
    or
    e
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r

    0
    .0
    9


    0
    .0
    9

    0
    .1
    0

    Le
    e

    (2
    0
    0
    1
    )

    D
    is
    se
    rt
    at
    io
    n

    8
    4
    (8
    5
    %
    )

    N
    R
    N
    u
    rs
    in
    g
    S
    W
    G
    C
    T
    A

    (0
    .7
    3
    )

    G
    P
    A
    /G
    ra
    d
    es
    >
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r

    0
    .1
    3

    0
    .1
    3
    0
    .1
    5

    M
    on

    ey
    (1
    9
    9
    7
    )

    D
    is
    se
    rt
    at
    io
    n

    6
    8
    (5

    9
    %

    )

    0
    %
    N
    u
    rs
    in
    g

    S
    C
    C
    T
    T
    (

    0
    .6
    2

    *)

    G
    P
    A
    /G
    ra
    d
    es
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r


    0
    .0
    1


    0
    .0
    1


    0
    .0
    2

    5
    6
    (5
    9
    %
    )

    0
    %
    N
    on
    -n
    u
    rs
    in
    g
    0
    .1
    3
    0
    .1
    3

    0
    .1
    7

    5
    7
    (5
    9
    %
    )

    0
    %

    0
    .3
    0

    0
    .3
    0

    0
    .3
    8

    P
    ar
    le
    tt

    (2
    0
    1
    2
    )

    D
    is
    se
    rt
    at
    io
    n

    4
    7
    (6
    7
    %
    )

    9
    %
    N
    u
    rs
    in
    g
    S
    M
    SL

    Q
    (0
    .7
    0
    *)

    G
    P
    A
    /G
    ra
    d
    es
    >
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r

    0
    .4
    9

    0
    .4
    9

    0
    .5
    8

    P
    it
    ts

    (2
    0
    0
    1
    )
    D
    is
    se
    rt
    at
    io
    n

    1
    6
    1
    (8
    5
    %
    )

    2
    5
    %

    N
    u
    rs
    in
    g
    S
    C
    C
    T
    ST

    (0
    .6
    2
    )

    G
    P
    A
    /G
    ra
    d
    es
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r

    0
    .4
    4

    0
    .3
    0

    0
    .3
    7

    >
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r

    0
    .3
    9

    6
    7
    (9
    0
    %
    )

    2
    6
    %

    D
    C
    C
    T
    D
    I
    (0
    .8
    8
    )

    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r
    0
    .1
    6
    >
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r
    0
    .1
    7
    S
    C
    C
    T
    ST
    (0
    .6
    2
    )
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r
    0
    .1
    3
    0
    .1
    4
    0
    .1
    5
    >
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r
    0
    .3
    0
    D
    C
    C
    T
    D
    I
    (0
    .8
    8
    )
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r

    0
    .0
    1
    >
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r
    0
    .1
    5

    2
    2
    8
    (8
    6
    %
    )

    2
    5
    %
    S
    C
    C
    T
    ST
    (0
    .6
    2
    )
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r
    0
    .3
    0

    0
    .2
    3

    0
    .3
    0
    >
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r

    0
    .3
    6

    D
    C
    C
    T
    D
    I
    (0
    .8
    8
    )
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r
    0
    .0
    9
    >
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r
    0
    .1
    7

    4
    9
    (8
    8
    %
    )

    4
    8
    %

    S
    C
    C
    T
    ST
    (0
    .6
    2
    )
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r

    0
    .3
    1

    0
    .3
    0

    0
    .3
    2

    >
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r

    0
    .3
    4

    D
    C
    C
    T
    D
    I
    (0
    .8
    8
    )
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r
    0
    .2
    8
    >
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r
    0
    .2
    8

    P
    u
    zz
    if
    er
    ro

    (2
    0
    0
    6
    )
    D
    is
    se
    rt
    at
    io
    n

    8
    1
    5
    (8
    0
    %
    )

    2
    6
    %
    N
    on
    -n
    u
    rs
    in
    g
    S
    M
    SL

    Q
    (0
    .8
    0
    )

    G
    P
    A
    /G
    ra
    d
    es
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r
    0
    .0
    0
    0
    .0
    0
    0
    .0
    0

    R
    ei
    d
    (2
    0
    0
    0
    )

    D
    is
    se
    rt
    at
    io
    n

    4
    1
    7
    (8
    6
    %
    )

    2
    4
    %

    N
    u
    rs
    in
    g
    S
    C
    C
    T
    ST

    (0
    .7
    1
    )

    G
    P
    A
    /G
    ra
    d
    es
    >
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r
    0
    .2
    2
    0
    .2
    2
    0
    .2
    6

    Si
    lv
    er

    (1
    9
    9
    9
    )

    D
    is
    se
    rt
    at
    io
    n

    3
    9
    8
    (7
    2
    %
    )

    N
    R
    N
    on
    -n
    u
    rs
    in
    g

    S
    SS

    SE
    S
    (0
    .8
    8
    )

    G
    P
    A
    /G
    ra
    d
    es
    >
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r

    0
    .6
    5

    0
    .6
    5
    0
    .6
    9

    Si
    n
    gl
    et
    on

    -W
    il
    li
    am

    s
    (2
    0
    0
    9
    )

    D
    is
    se
    rt
    at
    io
    n

    8
    5
    (6
    8
    %
    )

    6
    7
    %
    N
    on
    -n
    u
    rs
    in
    g
    S
    M
    SL
    Q
    (0
    .8
    0
    )
    G
    P
    A
    /G
    ra
    d
    es
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r
    0
    .0
    9
    0
    .0
    9
    0
    .1
    0

    Si
    su
    n
    g
    (2
    0
    0
    5
    )

    D
    is
    se
    rt
    at
    io
    n

    1
    0
    1
    5
    (8
    3
    %
    )

    6
    %
    N
    on
    -n
    u
    rs
    in
    g

    S
    C
    C
    A
    P
    (0
    .8
    2
    )

    T
    es
    t
    sc
    or
    e
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r
    0
    .3
    0
    0
    .3
    0

    0
    .3
    3

    T
    h
    om

    p
    so
    n
    (2
    0
    0
    6
    )

    D
    is
    se
    rt
    at
    io
    n

    1
    8
    5
    (8
    6
    %
    )

    N
    R
    N
    u
    rs
    in
    g

    S
    N
    E
    T
    (0
    .9
    3
    *)

    G
    P
    A
    /G
    ra
    d
    es
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r

    0
    .4
    8

    0
    .4
    8
    0
    .5
    0
    T
    h
    om

    p
    so
    n
    (2
    0
    0
    9
    )

    D
    is
    se
    rt
    at
    io
    n

    3
    2
    (N

    R
    )

    N
    R
    N
    u
    rs
    in
    g

    D
    C
    C
    T
    D
    I
    (0
    .9
    3
    )

    G
    P
    A
    /G
    ra
    d
    es
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r
    0
    .6
    2
    0
    .6
    2

    0
    .6
    4

    Y
    os
    t
    (2
    0
    0
    3
    )

    D
    is
    se
    rt
    at
    io
    n

    4
    0
    (9
    0
    %
    )

    N
    R
    N
    u
    rs
    in
    g
    S
    M
    SL
    Q
    (0
    .8
    0
    )
    G
    P
    A
    /G
    ra
    d
    es
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r
    0
    .0
    6
    0
    .0
    5
    0
    .0
    5
    T
    es
    t
    sc
    or
    e
    1
    se
    m
    es
    te
    r

    0
    .1
    1

    N
    ot
    e.

    A
    u
    th
    or

    n
    am

    es
    ar
    e
    li
    m
    it
    ed

    to
    ju
    st

    th
    e
    su
    rn
    am

    e
    of

    th
    e
    fi
    rs
    t
    au

    th
    or
    ;
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    fu
    ll
    au

    th
    or

    li
    st
    ,
    p
    le
    as
    e
    re
    fe
    r
    to

    th
    e
    re
    fe
    re
    n
    ce

    li
    st
    .
    C
    T
    =

    C
    ri
    ti
    ca
    l
    T
    h
    in
    ki
    n
    g;

    N
    R
    =

    n
    ot

    re
    p
    or
    te
    d
    ;
    D
    =

    D
    is
    p
    os
    it
    io
    n
    ;
    S
    =

    Sk

    il
    ls

    ;
    C
    C
    A
    P
    =

    C
    ol
    le
    gi
    at
    e

    A
    ss
    es
    sm

    en
    t
    of

    A
    ca
    d
    em

    ic
    P
    ro
    fi
    ci
    en

    cy
    ;
    C
    C
    T
    D
    I
    =

    C
    al
    if
    or
    n
    ia

    C
    ri
    ti
    ca
    l
    T
    h
    in
    ki
    n
    g
    D
    is
    p
    os
    it
    io
    n
    In
    ve

    n
    to
    ry
    ;
    C
    C
    T
    ST

    =
    C
    al
    if
    or
    n
    ia

    C
    ri
    ti
    ca
    l
    T
    h
    in
    ki
    n
    g
    Sk

    il
    ls

    T
    es
    t;
    C
    C
    T
    T
    =

    C
    or
    n
    el
    l
    C
    ri
    ti
    ca
    l
    T
    h
    in
    ki
    n
    g
    T
    es
    t;
    C
    T
    A
    =

    C
    ri
    ti
    ca
    l
    T
    h
    in
    ki
    n
    g
    A
    ss
    es
    sm

    en
    t;

    M
    SL

    Q
    =

    M
    ot
    iv
    at
    ed

    St
    ra
    te
    gi
    es

    fo
    r
    Le

    ar
    n
    in
    g
    Q
    u
    es
    ti
    on

    n
    ai
    re
    ;
    N
    E
    T
    =

    N
    u
    rs
    e
    E
    n
    tr
    an

    ce
    T
    es
    t;
    N
    JT

    R
    S
    =

    N
    ew

    Je
    rs
    ey

    T
    es
    t
    of

    R
    ea
    so
    n
    in
    g
    Sk

    il
    ls
    ;
    SS

    SE
    S
    =

    St
    u
    d
    y
    Sk

    il
    ls

    Se
    lf
    -e
    ffi
    ca
    cy

    Su
    rv
    ey

    (T
    ex
    t-
    ba

    se
    d
    C
    ri
    ti
    ca
    l
    T
    h
    in
    ki
    n
    g)
    ;
    T
    O
    LT

    =
    T
    es
    t
    of

    Lo
    gi
    ca
    l
    T
    h
    in
    ki
    n
    g;

    W
    G
    C
    T
    A
    =

    W
    at
    so
    n
    G
    la
    se
    r
    C
    ri
    ti
    ca
    l
    T
    h
    in
    ki
    n
    g
    A
    p
    p
    ra
    is
    al
    s.

    R
    el
    ia
    bi
    li
    ti
    es

    of
    th
    e
    sc
    al
    es

    ar
    e

    p
    ro
    vi
    d
    ed

    in
    p
    ar
    en

    th
    es
    es

    af
    te
    r
    sc
    al
    e
    ab

    br
    ev

    ia
    ti
    on

    s.
    W
    h
    en

    re
    li
    ab

    il
    it
    ie
    s
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    in
    cl
    u
    d
    ed

    st
    u
    d
    y
    sa
    m
    p
    le

    w
    er
    e
    n
    ot

    p
    ro
    vi
    d
    ed

    ,
    re
    li
    ab

    il
    it
    ie
    s
    fr
    om

    th
    e
    ci
    te
    d
    li
    te
    ra
    tu
    re

    w
    er
    e
    in
    cl
    u
    d
    ed

    as
    es
    ti
    m
    at
    es

    (t
    h
    es
    e
    ar
    e
    m
    ar
    ke

    d
    by

    an
    *)
    .

    r
    =

    w
    ei
    gh

    te
    d
    co

    rr
    el
    at
    io
    n
    fo
    r
    ea
    ch

    u
    n
    iq
    u
    e
    sa
    m
    p
    le
    ;
    ru

    =
    u
    n
    at
    te
    n
    u
    at
    ed

    w
    ei
    gh
    te
    d
    co
    rr
    el
    at
    io
    n
    fo
    r
    ea
    ch

    u
    n
    iq
    u
    e
    sa
    m
    p
    le
    .

    C.J. Fong et al. Thinking Skills and Creativity 26 (2017) 71–83
    76

    3.1. Characteristics of included studies

    In this section, we want to highlight salient characteristics of our pool of included studies (see Table 2 for the comprehensive list
    of characteristics of included studies). The first notable characteristic is the large majority of unpublished studies, namely doctoral
    dissertations, in our database of studies. Second, regarding student characteristics, there was a large proportion of nursing majors in
    the included samples. Third, measures of critical thinking mostly assessed skills, and only a few studies assessed dispositions. Some
    studies used measures that asked students to self-report their perceptions of their confidence to employ critical thinking skills (e.g.,
    Motivated Strategies Learning Questionnaire, MSLQ). In contrast, other instruments consisted of test questions (e.g., California
    Critical Thinking Skills Test), which judge their ability to perform on multiple-choice items. All instruments were established scales,
    validated by previous research. Lastly, academic achievement outcomes presented in the included studies were course grades or GPA,
    with a smaller segment of studies using individual exams as the dependent variable. Outcomes were assessed at the end of a semester
    (the duration of one course) or beyond one semester.

    3.2. Overall analysis of the relationship between community college and achievement

    First, we examined the overall relationship between critical thinking and community college student achievement (see Table 3).
    Under a fixed effects model, the weighted average r was 0.26 with a 95% CI of 0.23–0.28. Under a random effects model, the
    weighted average r was 0.24 with a 95% CI of 0.15–0.33. Therefore, the hypothesis that the relationship between critical thinking

    Table 3
    Overall Meta-Analysis Results of Critical Thinking and Community College Achievement.

    Fixed Effects Random Effects

    k r 95% CI k r 95% CI Qw

    Critical Thinking 27 0.26*** 0.23, 0.28 27 0.24*** 0.15, 0.33 247.12***
    Trim-and-Fill (FE) 30 0.27*** 0.25, 0.30 30 0.28*** 0.19, 0.36 2

    79

    .03***
    Trim-and-Fill (RE) 32 0.32*** 0.29, 0.34 32 0.30*** 0.21, 0.38 368.79***

    Note. Trim-and-fill procedures were conducted using both fixed effects (FE) and random effects (RE); trimmed studies were to the right of the mean. ***p < 0.001.

    Fig. 2. Forest plot of studies. The size of points reflects weight in meta-analysis.

    C.J. Fong et al. Thinking Skills and Creativity 26 (2017) 71–83

    77

    and achievement is equal to zero was rejected under both FE and RE models (p < 0.001). See Fig. 2 for forest plot. When meta- analyzing the unattenuated correlations (the adjusted effect sizes correcting for measurement error using scale reliabilities), the weighted average effect size was 0.28. The psychometric analysis would estimate the proportion of explained variance is 0.74, indicating that 74% of the total variance in the attenuated effect sizes is explained by sampling and measurement error.

    Using trim-and-fill analyses to look for asymmetry using both fixed- and random-error models (Duval & Tweedie, 2000), we
    searched for possible missing effects on the right side of the distribution, which would increase the size of the slightly positive average
    r for community college students on achievement. Using the fixed-effects model, we found evidence that three effect sizes might have
    been missing on the right side. Imputing these values would change the correlation between critical thinking and achievement to
    r = 0.27 (95% CI = 0.25, 0.30) under fixed effects and r = 0.28 (95% CI = 0.19, 0.36) under random effects. Using the random-
    effects model, we found evidence that five effect sizes might have been missing on the right side. Imputing these values would change
    the correlation between critical thinking and achievement to r = 0.32 (95% CI = 0.29, 0.34) under fixed effects and r = 0.30 (95%
    CI = 0.21, 0.38) under random effects. Thus, when accounting for possible data censoring, the relationship between critical thinking
    and community college student achievement is slightly stronger.

    In addition, the tests of the distribution of the effect sizes revealed that we could reject the hypothesis that the effects were
    estimating the same underlying population value, Q(26) = 247.12, p < 0.001; therefore, there was sufficient heterogeneity among the effect sizes that could be explained by moderator variables, which we will examine in the following section.

    3.3. Moderator analyses

    Next, we examined two categories of moderators: categorical moderators, which include publication status (published vs. un-
    published), student program/major (nursing vs. non-nursing), predictor type (skills vs. disposition), and outcome type (test score vs.
    GPA) and duration (one semester vs. beyond one semester); and continuous moderators which include percent female composition
    and percent minority student composition (see Table 4 for categorical moderator results). These continuous moderators were cal-
    culated because effect sizes were not often provided by gender or ethnicity. Therefore, we created continuous variables of the
    percentage of the sample that was female or minority status as a proxy for these demographic variables.

    For the first categorical moderator, we found no significant differences between published and unpublished studies, indicating
    that there does not seem to be a publication bias in this literature under both fixed and random effects (FE: Q = 0.35, p = 0.55; RE:
    Q = 0.11, p = 0.75). See Fig. 3 for funnel plot. Second, we assessed differences between nursing (k = 14) and non-nursing students
    (k = 13); non-nursing students consisted of collegians from business, music, computer science, and biology. Under both fixed and
    random effects, we did not observe a significant difference between the two groups of students (FE: Q = 1.86, p = 0.17; RE:
    Q = 0.13, p = 0.72). Third, we assessed whether the relationship between critical thinking and achievement would significantly
    differ if critical thinking was measured as skills (k = 24) or a disposition (k = 4). Moderator tests revealed no significant differences,
    under both fixed (Q = 0.62, p = 0.43) and random effects (Q = 0.76, p = 0.38). Fourth, we assessed if self-reported measures versus
    formal assessments moderated the relationship between students’ levels of critical thinking and academic achievement. Under fixed
    effects only, there was a significant difference between self-reported measures of critical thinking (r = 0.21) and ability tests of
    critical thinking (r = 0.28), Q = 6.48, p = 0.011. A significant difference was not detected under random effects, Q = 0.43,

    Table 4
    Categorical Moderator Analyses for Critical Thinking and Community College Achievement.

    Fixed Effects Random Effects

    k r Qb 95% CI r Qb 95% CI

    Publication Status 0.35 0.11
    Published 2 0.21*** 0.07, 0.35 0.21*** 0.07, 0.35
    Unpublished 27 0.26** 0.23, 0.28 0.24*** 0.15, 0.34

    Critical Thinking Type 0.62 0.76
    Skills 24 0.25*** 0.23, 0.28 0.23*** 0.13, 0.32
    Disposition 4 0.31*** 0.16, 0.45 0.33** 0.11, 0.53

    Type of Measure 6.48* 0.54
    Self-reported 11 0.21*** 0.16, 0.25 0.18 −0.06, 0.40
    Ability test 16 0.28*** 0.25, 0.31 0.27*** 0.20, 0.34

    Major/Program 1.86 0.13
    Nursing 17 0.23*** 0.19, 0.28 0.22*** 0.14, 0.31
    Non-nursing 13 0.27*** 0.24, 0.30 0.26*** 0.09, 0.40

    Outcome Type 0.03 0.79
    GPA/Grades 25 0.26*** 0.23, 0.29 00.25*** 0.05, 0.20
    Tests/Exams 3 0.25*** 0.20, 0.31 0.10 −0.21, 0.40

    Outcome Duration 34.05*** 0.65
    One semester 22 0.18*** 0.14, 0.22 0.21*** 0.11, 0.31
    > one semester 11 0.33*** 0.30, 0.37 0.28*** 0.14, 0.42

    Note. Sample sizes varied per analysis due to the shifting unit of analysis approach and the existence of multiple effect sizes per study; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

    C.J. Fong et al. Thinking Skills and Creativity 26 (2017) 71–83
    78

    p = 0.46, but mean point estimates were trending in a similar direction to the fixed effects model.
    Fifth, we compared the effects of critical thinking on individual test scores such as nursing licensure exams (k = 3) with overall

    GPA (k = 24), which also resulted in nonsignificant differences (FE: Q = 0.03, p = 0.87; RE: Q = 0.79, p = 0.38). Lastly, we tested
    outcome duration as a moderator variable, one semester (k = 22) compared to beyond one semester (k = 11). Under fixed effects
    only, there was a significant difference with a larger average effect size for achievement outcomes beyond one semester (r = 0.33)
    and a smaller average effect size for outcomes at one semester (r = 0.18), Q = 34.06, p < 0.001. A significant difference was not detected under random effects, Q = 0.65, p = 0.41, but the observed mean effect sizes were in a similar direction to the fixed effects model. Forest plots for categorical moderator analyses are present in supplementary materials.

    For the continuous moderators, we found significant moderation of gender (k = 25) and minority status (k = 17) under fixed
    effects only. Specifically, using the percentage of females in the student sample composition, we found a slope value for gender on
    effect sizes significantly different from zero (FE: β = 0.25, p = 0.02, RE: β = 0.29, p = 0.42). This result suggests that as female
    percentage increases, the relation of critical thinking and achievement is higher. In addition, using the percentage of historically
    underrepresented minority students (non-White, non-Asian) in the student sample composition, we found a slope value for minority
    status on effect sizes significantly different from zero (FE: β = −0.27, p = 0.02, RE: β = −0.09, p = 0.68). Thus, as minority status
    percentage increases, the relation of critical thinking and achievement was lower. However, due to incomplete data reporting in the
    primary studies, the sample sizes of these moderator tests were reduced, and results should be interpreted with caution.

    In sum, critical thinking had a moderate influence on community college achievement across a variety of student programs and
    outcome types with no sign of publication bias. Under fixed effects only, it appeared that critical thinking was more strongly related
    to longer-term achievement outcomes beyond the one semester mark. Examining demographic characteristics as moderators revealed
    that critical thinking seemed more impactful for women and majority students but under fixed effects only.

    4. Discussion

    In sum, the results of our meta-analysis indicated positive relationships between critical thinking and community college student
    success. According to Cohen’s (1992) benchmarks for evaluating effect sizes, the magnitude of the relationships was small to
    moderate. Additionally, when conducting sensitivity analyses to impute missing effect sizes and correct for attenuated correlations
    affected by measurement error and internal consistency of critical thinking scales, the truer weighted average effect sizes was even a
    bit larger. Given the low rates of degree completion that plague U.S. two-year colleges and the complexity of community college
    persistence and achievement, identifying positive factors, such as critical thinking, that are malleable and can bolster student success
    is surely worthwhile. Not accounting for other background characteristics, critical thinking skills and dispositions seem to positively
    relate with community college student success.

    Comparing the relationship between critical thinking and achievement from previous meta-analytic reviews on four-year college
    or university students, we observed that the average weighted correlations in our meta-analysis were markedly higher. In a meta-
    analysis by Robbins, Lauver, Le, Davis, and Langley (2004), the observed mean correlation between academic-related skills (aca-
    demic and cognitive strategies) and GPA was 0.129. Similarly, in an updated review by Richardson, Abraham, and Bond (2012), the
    observed average correlation between critical thinking and achievement (correcting for sampling error) was 0.15. The effects in our
    sample of community college studies ranged from 0.24 to 0.32. In our community college sample, the critical thinking-achievement
    relationship was twice the magnitude. One may argue that compared to the four-year university setting, attaining higher achievement

    Fig. 3. Funnel plot of studies.

    C.J. Fong et al. Thinking Skills and Creativity 26 (2017) 71–83
    79

    at community colleges requires students to develop higher levels of critical thinking because of the increased demands of work and
    family responsibilities. Terenzini et al. (1996) suggested that college students who work longer hours off-campus and juggle multiple
    responsibilities (like many community college students) can cultivate better time management skills, self-discipline, and academic
    focus, which in turn lead to greater cognitive development and higher levels of critical thinking.

    Furthermore, because critical thinking development has been found because of the significant vocational focus at the community
    college level, perhaps critical thinking is more relevant to success as students directly engage in workforce preparation. For instance,
    in community colleges, nurses-in-training, which comprised a large portion of the meta-analytic student sample, are taught to reason
    critically about the judgments they make in their day-to-day responsibilities and the life-and-death decisions in nursing practice
    (Colucciello, 1997). Thus, the demand for high levels of critical thinking for the success of community college nursing students may
    explain the larger associations in our meta-analysis compared to previous reviews.

    Moderator results. Results from our moderator tests revealed interesting patterns regarding the demographic characteristics of
    the community college samples as well as measurement and methodology issues. Before interpreting our moderator results, one
    caveat to understanding the significance level of the contrasts is when there are discrepant findings between fixed and random effects.
    When this occurs, the random effects model is considered more robust, so significant findings solely under fixed effects should be
    interpreted with caution. In general, moderator results in meta-analysis are exploratory given the nature of across-study synthesis.
    Also, we reiterate the caution required when interpreting results from these exploratory tests given the small number of studies that
    comprise moderator categories.

    Regarding student characteristics, critical thinking was not moderated by nursing student status, indicating that critical thinking
    influences achievement equally for nursing and non-nursing disciplines. In spite of the emphasis of critical thinking in nursing
    education, critical thinking seemed to be equally important across majors. Under fixed effects, taking into consideration gender and
    minority status, evidence that the relationship between students’ levels of critical thinking and achievement decreased with samples
    of greater male representation and minority representation is noteworthy. With regards to small correlations for students with greater
    numbers of minority students, this result leads to an interesting discussion on the impact of racial diversity and diversity experiences
    on cognitive development. From the literature, one may expect that as minority composition increases, critical thinking would be
    further cultivated and lead to increased achievement due to the positive influence of diversity experiences on students’ levels of
    critical thinking (Bowman, 2010). One possible explanation to disentangle this effect comes from research that demonstrated di-
    versity experiences to be more impactful for White students’ levels of critical thinking skills compared to non-White students (Loes,
    Pascarella, & Umbach, 2012). Although samples with a greater minority student composition, in theory, have more opportunities for
    interpersonal diversity experiences, the minority students themselves may not have as a large of a benefit on their cognitive de-
    velopment and thus academic achievement.

    Although these findings were only apparent under fixed effects, it is interesting that the achievement of minority males did not
    benefit as much from critical thinking, especially given the attention focused on their remediation in the community college lit-
    erature. Harris and Wood (2014) developed the Socio-Ecological Outcomes model to capture the variety of factors that come into play
    regarding male minority student success in community colleges. Factors from the individual, academic and environmental contexts,
    and campus ethos (and the interactions among them) are proposed to increase student success. Thus, a multitude of other factors
    apart from critical thinking skills and dispositions are required to understand all community college students and their success,
    particularly minority male collegians.

    From a methodological perspective, moderator tests indicated that skills and dispositions were equally related with community
    college achievement, under both fixed and random effects. However, given the few studies that measured dispositions, further
    research in this area is required. One significant methodological moderator under fixed effects only involved whether critical thinking
    was measured via a self-report survey or ability test, with the latter having stronger associations with academic achievement. This
    finding suggests that ability tests are potentially more valid when related with achievement, as self-reported surveys may introduce
    more measurement error. Regarding outcome characteristics, the relationship between critical thinking and achievement was not
    moderated by achievement type (GPA/grades or tests/exams); similarly, the small number of studies contributing effect sizes for tests
    and exams warrants additional scholarship. Another significant moderator result in our study (under fixed effects) revealed that effect
    sizes were larger when achievement was measured beyond one semester compared to at the one semester mark. This suggests that
    critical thinking had a stronger relationship with achievement in the long run, which can be possibly explained through the cu-
    mulative effects of psychological strategies which develop and strengthen over time (Dent & Koenka, 2016).

    Publication bias. Although the moderator analysis comparing published and unpublished studies did not indicate a publication
    bias (given the few published studies in our sample), the more notable result is the large majority of unpublished studies in our
    dataset. This is indicative of the field of community college research. Crisp, Carales, and Nuñez (2016) observed how despite the long
    history of community colleges in the U.S., only since the 1990s have research in this area emerged. They recognized that the majority
    of this research may be unpublished, and our study corroborates such findings along with previous meta-analyses (Fong et al., 2017).
    Whereas some scholars may scrutinize the heavy use of unpublished data in a meta-analysis citing lack of quality as the primary
    criticism, the accepted and encouraged practice in contemporary meta-analysis in the education field is to include all viable data
    regardless of publication status (Polanin et al., 2016). This can aid in mitigating the threat of publication bias.

    4.1. Implications for research and practice

    This study has several important implications for research and practice, but for the purposes of this study, we focus on a few
    salient points. First, regarding implications for future research, our systematic synthesis revealed a few gaps in the literature. For

    C.J. Fong et al. Thinking Skills and Creativity 26 (2017) 71–83

    80

    example, our search through the literature exposed a dearth of studies that focus on other important community college outcomes
    such as persistence, degree attainment, transfer, and job placement and the role of critical thinking. Only one study (Eason, 1986)
    measured credits earned and course completion as an outcome in relation to critical thinking with small or negative correlations
    ranging from −0.04 to 0.05. Further research is needed to examine persistence-related outcomes beyond achievement and test
    outcomes.

    By affirming the positive relationship between critical thinking and community college success, this study offers administrators
    and practitioners accumulated and synthesized evidence for informed decision-making and practices. In particular, our findings can
    be used to build programs, initiatives, orientations, success courses, and trainings that focus on bolstering critical thinking in
    community college students (Piergiovanni, 2014). Given the substantive relationship between students’ levels of critical thinking and
    community college student success, one suggestion is to teach students the importance of critical thinking and its relevance for
    academic achievement. Previous research has underscored the use of critical thinking courses to further cognitive development
    (Mayhew et al., 2016). These courses along with other opportunities can serve as the context for faculty to discuss with students their
    understanding of critical thinking, its bearing on their academic performance, and how it can be developed further. Additionally,
    critical thinking can also be infused in instructional practices as recommended by meta-analysis, which emphasized two proven
    strategies to increase critical thinking. First, opportunities of discussion or dialogue where teachers pose questions in groups or
    whole-class instruction were helpful. Second, exposing students to authentic tasks with role playing or applied problem solving
    promoted critical thinking. Third, in combination with these two strategies, including mentorship as an additional strategy appeared
    to augment the critical thinking process. Furthermore, when measuring students’ levels of critical thinking, formal ability tests like
    the Watson Glaser Critical Thinking Appraisal may be more preferable given the stronger associations between tests and achievement
    compared to using self-reported surveys.

    However, from our moderator results, extra supports beyond critical thinking training need to be offered for collegiate men and
    students of color who may not benefit as much from boosts in critical thinking. In general, the small to moderate correlations within
    this study, especially the smaller correlations for samples with greater proportions of male and minority students, demonstrate the
    need to examine other factors that influence community college achievement.

    Finally, we hope our study provides an example and encouragement for the use of meta-analysis as a methodological tool to
    understand the landscape of higher education research. Systematic review and synthesis techniques can afford researchers, admin-
    istrators, educators, and practitioners the ability to confidently evaluate, measure, target, and change the most important factors
    related to community college student outcomes.

    4.2. Limitations to generalizability

    There were a number of limitations to this study. The first limitation of all research syntheses in general consist of synthesis-
    generated evidence, which should not be interpreted as supporting causal relationships (Cooper, 2010). For example, it is not ne-
    cessarily the case of our meta-analysis that critical thinking causes higher achievement because high achieving students may possess
    greater critical thinking. Therefore, when significant differences were found within a research synthesis, results should be interpreted
    with caution and used to direct future research of these factors in a controlled design to appropriately infer causal impact. It is also
    important to recognize that the findings were based on small numbers of effect sizes, making it difficult to place great confidence in
    the magnitude of the estimated effects.

    In addition, meta-analysts depend on the authors of the primary studies to provide all relevant data and characteristics. Thus,
    there were many important variables that could not be examined as moderators due to incomplete reporting. Sample characteristics
    such as age, full-time status, hours working, and distance versus traditional learning modes (which are very significant factors when
    studying community college students) could not be assessed. One important research design issue that was not inferable from the
    included studies was the timeframe of critical thinking assessment. A researcher could foreseeably assess critical thinking at the
    beginning of community college as a predictor for success or towards the end of their schooling. The latter situation would be
    confounded with the influence of postsecondary instructive over the duration of the college experience (Huber & Kuncel, 2016).
    Future research is encouraged to systematically examine these factors to see their moderating influence on the relationship between
    students’ levels of critical thinking and postsecondary student success.

    Another limitation of the study is the psychometric approach towards operationalizing critical thinking taken in this study. Liu,
    Frankel, and Roohr (2014) argued for a parsimony between authenticity and psychometric quality, citing that measures that rely on
    multiple-choice tests are not fully capturing the complexities of critical thinking. Rather, measures that prioritize psychometric
    quality may be tapping into other related constructs such as reading comprehension or general linguistic competencies. Although the
    current study has solely focused on these types of measures, we encourage future research to examine other ways of understanding
    and measuring critical thinking that reflects the authenticity of both the construct and the contexts in which students use their critical
    thinking skills.

    Appendix A. Supplementary data

    Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, in the online version, at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2017.06.
    002.

    C.J. Fong et al. Thinking Skills and Creativity 26 (2017) 71–83
    81

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2017.06.002

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2017.06.002

    References1

    Abrami, P. C., Bernard, R. M., Borokhovski, E., Wade, A., Surkes, M. A., Tamim, R., et al. (2008). Instructional interventions affecting critical thinking skills and
    dispositions: A stage 1 meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 78, 1102–1134. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0034654308326084.

    Arum, R., & Roksa, J. (2011). Academically adrift: limited learning on college campuses. University of Chicago Press.
    *Bachman, M. L. (1998). Anxiety, critical thinking and age as performance predictors of community college nursing students. CO: Colorado State University Fort Collins

    [Unpublished doctoral dissertation].
    Bers, T. H., McGowan, M., & Rubin, A. (1996). The disposition to think critically among community college students: The California critical thinking dispositions

    inventory. The Journal of General Education, 45, 197–223.
    Borenstein, M., Hedges, L., Higgins, J., & Rothstein, H. (2005). Comprehensive meta-analysis (Version 2.1) [Computer software]. Englewood, NJ: BioStat.
    Borenstein, M., Hedges, L., Higgins, J., & Rothstein, H. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. West Sussex, UK: Wiley & Sons.
    Bowman, N. A. (2010). College diversity experiences and cognitive development: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 80, 4–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.

    3102/0034654309352495.
    Bowman, N. A. (2012). Effect sizes and statistical methods for meta-analysis in higher education. Research in Higher Education, 53, 375–382. http://dx.doi.org/10.

    1007/s11162-011-9232-5.
    Brint, S., Cantwell, A. M., & Saxena, P. (2012). Disciplinary categories majors, and undergraduate academic experiences: Rethinking Bok’s underachieving colleges

    thesis. Research in Higher Education, 53, 1–25.
    Brookfield, S. D. (1987). Developing critical thinkers. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
    Calcagno, J., Bailey, T., Jenkins, D., Kienzl, G., & Leinbach, D. T. (2006). IIs student right-to-know all you should know? An analysis of community college graduation

    rates. Research in Higher Education, 47, 491–519. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11162-005-9005-0.
    Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155.
    Colucciello, M. L. (1997). Critical thinking skills and dispositions of baccalaureate nursing students: A conceptual model for evaluation. Journal of Professional Nursing,

    13, 236–245.
    Cooper, H., Hedges, L. V., & Valentine, J. C. (Vol. Eds.), (2009). The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis. 1Russell Sage Foundation.
    Cooper, H. (2010). Synthesizing research: A guide for literature reviews (4th ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
    *Cornelius, S. T. (2011). Prior grade point average, social and financial support, and standardized test scores as predictors of success among associates degree nursing students

    (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Ann Arbor, MI: Walden University.
    *Criner, L. A. (1992). Teaching thinking and reasoning: A study of critical thinking in adults (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Fayetteville, AR: University of Arkansas.
    Crisp, G., Carales, V. D., & Núñez, A. M. (2016). Where is the research on community college students. Community College Journal of Research and Practice, 40, 767–778.
    Dent, A. L., & Koenka, A. C. (2016). The relation between self-regulated learning and academic achievement across childhood and adolescence: A meta-analysis.

    Educational Psychology Review, 28, 425–474. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9320-8.
    Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000). A nonparametric trim and fill method of accounting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Journal of the American Statistical Association,

    95, 89–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01621459.2000.10473905.
    *Eason, L. E. (1986). The relationship of critical thinking skills and psychological type in community college students’ responses to science instruction (Unpublished doctoral

    dissertation). Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.
    Ennis, R. H., Millman, J., & Tomko, T. N. (1985). Cornell critical thinking tests level X & level Z: Manual. Pacific Grove, CA: Midwest Publications.
    Facione, N. C., Facione, P. A., & Sanchez, C. A. (1994). Critical thinking disposition as a measure of competent clinical judgment: The development of the California

    Critical Thinking Disposition Inventory. Journal of Nursing Education, 33(8), 345–350.
    Facione, P. A., Facione, N. C., & Giancarlo, C. A. F. (2001). California critical thinking disposition inventory: CCTDI. California Academic Press.
    Facione, P. A. (2009). Critical thinking: What it is and why it counts. [Retrieved from http://www.insightassessment.com/9articles%20WW.html].
    Fike, D. S., & Fike, R. (2008). Predictors of first-year student retention in the community college. Community College Review, 36, 68–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/

    0091552108320222.
    Fong, C. J., Davis, C. W., Kim, Y., Kim, Y. W., Marriott, L., & Kim, S. (2017). Psychosocial factors and community college success: A meta-analytic investigation. Review

    of Educational Research, 87, 388–424. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0034654316653479.
    *Foust, R. A. (2008). Learning strategies, motivation, and self-reported academic outcomes of students enrolled in web-based coursework (Unpublished doctoral dissertation)

    Detroit, MI: Wayne State University.
    Garrison, D. R. (1992). Critical thinking and self-directed learning in adult education: an analysis of responsibility and control issues. Adult Education Quarterly, 42(3),

    136–148.
    *Gaythwaite, E. S. (2006). Metacognitive self-regulation, self-efficacy for learning and performance, and critical thinking as predictors of academic success and course retention

    among community college students enrolled in online, telecourse, and traditional public speaking courses (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Orlando, FL: University of
    Central Florida.

    Gellin, A. (2003). The effect of undergraduate student involvement on critical thinking: A meta-analysis of the literature 1991–2000. Journal of College Student
    Development, 44, 746–762. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/csd.2003.0066.

    Giancarlo, C. A., & Facione, P. A. (2001). A look across four years at the disposition toward critical thinking among undergraduate students. Journal of General
    Education, 50, 29–55.

    Goldrick-Rab, S. (2010). Challenges and opportunities for improving community college student success. Review of Educational Research, 80, 437–469. http://dx.doi.
    org/10.3102/0034654310370163.

    Greenhouse, J. B., & Iyengar, S. (1994). Sensitivity analysis and diagnosis. In H. Cooper, & S. Iyengar (Eds.), Handbook of research synthesis (pp. 383–398). New York:
    Russell Sage Foundation.

    *Guster, D., & Batt, R. (1989). Cognitive and affective variables and their relationships to performance in a lotus 1–2-3 class. Collegiate Microcomputer, 7, 151–156.
    Halpern, D. F. (1998). Teaching critical thinking for transfer across domains: Dispositions, skills, structure training, and metacognitive monitoring. American

    Psychologist, 53, 449–455. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.53.4.449.
    Harris, F., III, & Wood, J. L. (2014). The socio-ecological outcomes model: A framework for examining men of color’s experiences and success in community colleges. San Diego,

    CA: Minority Male Community College Collaborative.
    *Hearron, M. C. (1991). The predictive effect of logical thinking, prior knowledge, and learning style characteristics on academic achievement in an anatomy and physiology

    course (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Commerce, TX: East Texas State University.
    Hedges, L. V., & Vevea, J. L. (1998). Fixed-and random-effects models in meta-analysis. Psychological Methods, 3, 486–504.
    Heijltjes, A., Van Gog, T., Leppink, J., & Paas, F. (2014). Improving critical thinking: Effects of dispositions and instructions on economics students’ reasoning skills.

    Learning and Instruction, 29, 31–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.07.003.
    Huber, C. R., & Kuncel, N. R. (2016). Does college teach critical thinking? A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 86, 431–468. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/

    0034654315605917.
    *Hurov, J. T. (1987). A study of the relationship between reading, computational, and critical thinking skills and academic success in fundamentals of chemistry (Report)St.

    Louis, MO: Saint Louis Community College.
    Jacobs, J., & Dougherty, K. J. (2006). The uncertain future of the community college workforce development mission. New Directions for Community Colleges,

    2006(136), 53–62.

    1 Denoted with an * were included in the meta-analysis.

    C.J. Fong et al. Thinking Skills and Creativity 26 (2017) 71–83
    82

    http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0034654308326084

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0010

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0015

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0020

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0025

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0030

    http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0034654309352495

    http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0034654309352495

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11162-011-9232-5

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11162-011-9232-5

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0045

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0050

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11162-005-9005-0

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0065

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0070

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0075

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0080

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0085

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0090

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9320-8

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01621459.2000.10473905

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0105

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0110

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0115

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0120

    http://www.insightassessment.com/9articles%20WW.html

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0091552108320222

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0091552108320222

    http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0034654316653479

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0140

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0145

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0150

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0150

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/csd.2003.0066

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0160

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0160

    http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0034654310370163

    http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0034654310370163

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0170

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0175

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.53.4.449

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0185

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0190

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0195

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.07.003

    http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0034654315605917

    http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0034654315605917

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0210

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0215

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0215

    King, P. M., Wood, P. K., & Mines, R. A. (1990). Critical thinking among college and graduate students. The Review of Higher Education, 13, 167–186.
    *Kuznar, K. A. (2009). Effects of high-fidelity human patient simulation experience on self-efficacy, motivation and learning of first semester associate degree nursing students.

    Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota.
    *Lee, G. J. (2001). An investigation of the development of critical thinking skills for traditional program and non-traditional program associate degree nursing students

    (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Moscow, ID: University of Idaho.
    Li, G., Long, S., & Simpson, M. E. (1999). Self-perceived gains in critical thinking and communication skills: Are there disciplinary differences. Research in Higher

    Education, 40, 43–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1018722327398.
    Liu, O. L., Frankel, L., & Roohr, K. C. (2014). Assessing critical thinking in higher education: Current state and directions for next-generation assessment. ETS Research

    Reports Series, 2014, 1–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ets2.12009.
    Loes, C., Pascarella, E., & Umbach, P. (2012). Effects of diversity experiences on critical thinking skills. Journal of Higher Education, 83, 1–25.
    Mayhew, M. J., Rockenbach, A. N., Bowman, N. A., Seifert, T. A. D., Wolniak, G. C., Pascarella, E. T., et al. (2016). How college affects students: 21st century evidence that

    higher education works, Vol. 3. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
    McMillan, J. H. (1987). Enhancing college students’ critical thinking: a review of studies. Research in Higher Education, 26, 3–29.
    Money, S. M. (1997). The Relationship Between Critical Thinking Scores, Achievement Scores, and Grade Point Average in Three Different Disciplines (Unpublished Doctoral

    Dissertation). Lansing MI: Michigan State University.
    Moore, T. J. (2013). Critical thinking: Seven definitions in search of a concept. Studies in Higher Education, 38, 506–522. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03075079.2011.

    586995.
    Overton, R. C. (1998). A comparison of fixed-effects and mixed (random-effects) models for meta-analysis tests of moderator variable effects. Psychological Methods,

    3(3), 354–379.
    *Parlett, D. K. (2012). A comparison of associate and bachelor degree nursing students’ motivation (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Minneapolis, MN: Walden University.
    Pascarella, E. T., & Terenzini, P. T. (2005). In K. A. Feldman (Vol. Ed.), How college affects students. Vol. 2. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
    Patall, E. A., Cooper, H., & Robinson, J. C. (2008). The effects of choice on intrinsic motivation and related outcomes: A meta-analysis of research findings.

    Psychological Bulletin, 134, 270–300. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.270.
    Piergiovanni, P. R. (2014). Creating a critical thinker. College Teaching, 62, 86–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/87567555.2014.896775.
    Pike, G. R., Kuh, G. D., & McCormick, A. C. (2011). An investigation of the contingent relationships between learning community participation and student en-

    gagement. Research in Higher Education, 52, 300–322.
    Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A., García, T., & McKeachie, W. J. (1993). Reliability and predictive validity of the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ).

    Educational and Psychological Measurement, 53(3), 801–813.
    Pithers, R. T., & Soden, R. (2000). Critical thinking in education: A review. Educational Research, 42, 237–249. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/001318800440579.
    *Pitts, L. N. (2001). Critical thinking skill and disposition as predictors of success in associate degree nursing education (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Gainesville, FL:

    University of Florida.
    Polanin, J. R., Tanner-Smith, E. E., & Hennessy, E. A. (2016). Estimating the difference between published and unpublished effect sizes: A meta-review. Review of

    Educational Research, 86, 207–236. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0034654315582067.
    *Puzziferro, M. (2006). Online technologies self-efficacy, self-regulated learning, and experiential variables as predictors of final grade and satisfaction in college-level online

    courses (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). New York, NY: New York University.
    *Reid, H. V. (2000). The correlation between a general critical thinking skills test and a discipline-specific critical thinking test for associate degree nursing students (Unpublished

    doctoral dissertation). Denton, TX: University of North Texas.
    Richardson, M., Abraham, C., & Bond, R. (2012). Psychological correlates of university students’ academic performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis.

    Psychological Bulletin, 138, 353–387. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0026838.
    Robbins, S. B., Lauver, K., Le, H., Davis, D., Langley, R., & Carlstrom, A. (2004). Do psychosocial and study skill factors predict college outcomes? A meta-analysis.

    Psychological Bulletin, 130, 261–288. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.2.261.
    Scheffer, B. K., & Rubenfeld, M. G. (2000). A consensus statement on critical thinking in nursing. Journal of Nursing Education, 39(8), 352–359.
    Shapiro, D., Dundar, A., Yuan, X., Harrell, A., & Wakhungu, P. K. (2014). Completing college: A national view of student attainment rates – fall 2008 cohort (Signature report

    No. 8)Herndon, VA: National Student Clearinghouse Research Center.
    *Silver, B. B. (1999). Indicators of academic achievement: A structural equation model (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Storrs, CT: The University of Connecticut.
    *Singleton-Williams, S. D. (2009). Motivational and cognitive learning strategies as predictors of academic success in economically disadvantaged community college students

    (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Minneapolis, MN: Capella University.
    *Sisung, J. (2005). Relationship between standardized critical thinking test scores and earned grades in courses purported to teach critical thinking at Kellogg Community College

    (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Lincoln, NE: The University of Nebraska.
    Remaking college: The changing ecology of higher education. In M. Stevens, & M. Kirst (Eds.), Stanford University Press.
    Terenzini, P. T., Springer, L., Pascarella, E. T., & Nora, A. (1995). Influences affecting the development of students’ critical thinking skills. Research in Higher Education,

    36, 23–39.
    Terenzini, P. T., Springer, L., Yaeger, P. M., Pascarella, E. T., & Nora, A. (1996). First-generation college students: Characteristics, experiences, and cognitive de-

    velopment. Research in Higher Education, 37, 1–22.
    *Thompson, M. (2006). A correlational study on critical thinking as a predictor of success in the associate degree nursing program at Seminole Community College (Unpublished

    doctoral dissertation). Minneapolis, MN: Capella University.
    *Thompson, J. (2009). To question or not to question: The effects of two teaching approaches on students’ thinking dispositions, critical thinking skills, and course grades in a

    critical thinking course (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Minneapolis, MN: Capella University.
    Walsh, C. M., & Hardy, R. C. (1999). Dispositional differences in critical thinking related to gender and academic major. Journal of Nursing Education, 38, 149–155.
    Watson, G., & Glaser, E. M. (1964). Watson-Glaser Critical Thinking Appraisal manual: Forms. New York, NY: Harcourt, Brace, and World.
    Wilson, D. G., & Wagner, E. E. (1981). The Watson-Glaser Critical Thinking Appraisal as a predictor of performance in a critical thinking course. Educational and

    Psychological Measurement, 41(4), 1319–1322.
    Xu, D., & Jaggars, S. S. (2011). Online and hybrid course enrollment and performance in Washington State community and technical colleges (Unpublished manuscript) (2011,

    March)NY: Community College Research Center, Columbia University New York.
    *Yost, H. D. (2003). Motivational orientation, self-regulated learning, and academic performance in associate degree nursing students (Unpublished doctoral dissertation).

    Greensboro, NC: The University of North Carolina.
    Zientek, L. R., Yetkiner, Z. E., Fong, C. J., & Griffin, M. (2013). Student success in developmental mathematics. Community College Journal of Research and Practice, 37,

    990–1010. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10668926.2010.491993.

    C.J. Fong et al. Thinking Skills and Creativity 26 (2017) 71–83
    83

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0220

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0225

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0230

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0230

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1018722327398

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ets2.12009

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0245

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0250

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0255

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0260

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0260

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03075079.2011.586995

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03075079.2011.586995

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0270

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0270

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0275

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0280

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.270

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/87567555.2014.896775

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0295

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0295

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0300

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0300

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/001318800440579

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0310

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0310

    http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/0034654315582067

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0320

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0320

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0325

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0325

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0026838

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.2.261

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0340

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0345

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0345

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0350

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0355

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0355

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0360

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0360

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0365

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0370

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0370

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0375

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0375

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0380

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0380

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0385

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0385

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0390

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0395

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0400

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0400

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0405

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0405

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0410

    http://refhub.elsevier.com/S1871-1871(16)30150-X/sbref0410

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10668926.2010.491993

    • A meta-analysis on critical thinking and community college student achievement
    • Introduction
      Critical thinking
      Critical thinking and community college students
      Factors related to critical thinking and college student achievement
      Research questions
      Method
      Literature search and inclusion criteria
      Information retrieval and coding
      Effect size calculation and data integration
      Results
      Characteristics of included studies
      Overall analysis of the relationship between community college and achievement
      Moderator analyses
      Discussion
      Implications for research and practice
      Limitations to generalizability
      Supplementary data
      References1

    Calculate your order
    275 words
    Total price: $0.00

    Top-quality papers guaranteed

    54

    100% original papers

    We sell only unique pieces of writing completed according to your demands.

    54

    Confidential service

    We use security encryption to keep your personal data protected.

    54

    Money-back guarantee

    We can give your money back if something goes wrong with your order.

    Enjoy the free features we offer to everyone

    1. Title page

      Get a free title page formatted according to the specifics of your particular style.

    2. Custom formatting

      Request us to use APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, or any other style for your essay.

    3. Bibliography page

      Don’t pay extra for a list of references that perfectly fits your academic needs.

    4. 24/7 support assistance

      Ask us a question anytime you need to—we don’t charge extra for supporting you!

    Calculate how much your essay costs

    Type of paper
    Academic level
    Deadline
    550 words

    How to place an order

    • Choose the number of pages, your academic level, and deadline
    • Push the orange button
    • Give instructions for your paper
    • Pay with PayPal or a credit card
    • Track the progress of your order
    • Approve and enjoy your custom paper

    Ask experts to write you a cheap essay of excellent quality

    Place an order